| |
Colloquium Presentation for Week 4 |
Colloquium Presenter: Dr. Larry Morton |
|
|
Morton's Presentation |
Find: |
|
It's about MEEMs, that is,
Media-Embedded/Embedded-Media Pedagogical Techniques. It's about
Cynicism. It's about differences, and trying to understand the determinants
of such differences... |
Abstract |
Abstract
Approximately 500 post baccalaureate students were asked
to respond to 11 pedagogical techniques to which they were exposed in a
media-friendly lecture hall. At times the techniques were embedded in media,
at other times the techniques had media embedded within them. A cynicism
measure was constructed from those who showed no positive response to a
particular method. This led to cynicism rates ranging from 20% to 62% for
the various methods used. WEB-oriented methods showed cynicism rates ranging
from approximately 28-42%. Ironically, while brief stories (using speech,
PowerPoint text and animation) generated the least amount of cynicism (20%),
an audio story by a classic story teller generated the most cynicism (62%).
A psychodynamic model incorporating information-intake styles,
information-expression styles, and demographics was constructed to examine
the determinants of such cynicism via Logistic Regression analyses. The
model was reliable for six of the 11 variables; and numerous predictor
variables revealed the complex interplay between pedagogical technique and
the type of student. Even with popular techniques like sound-bites,
PowerPoint, animation, MPEG, stories, and the use of the Internet, there was
a substantial rate of cynicism. However, in the spirit of
"multiple-perspective-taking" these cynicism rates may be viewed as a
positive phenomenon as well as a negative phenomenon. |
PowerPoint |
Presentation slides available for download here. |
|
For PowerPoint files you can go to Microsoft for the free viewer. |
Microsoft
Downloads |
|
Draft |
Draft version of the manuscript will be
sent out as an e-mail attachment in January. The draft is also posted
below for on-line readers. |
SEE BELOW! |
|
Discussion |
Discussion questions posted on WebCT site |
WebCT Discussion |
|
|
|
Find: |
|
|
Media-Embedded/Embedded-Media (MEEM)
Teaching Techniques and Student Cynicism
Dr. L. L. Morton
University of Windsor
Faculty of Education
401 Sunset Avenue
Windsor, Ontario
N9B3P4
e-mail morton@uwindsor.ca
January 27, 2002
Abstract
Approximately 500 post baccalaureate students were asked to respond to 11
pedagogical techniques to which they were exposed in a media-friendly
lecture hall. At times the techniques were embedded in media, at other times
the techniques had media embedded within them. A cynicism measure was
constructed from those who showed no positive response to a particular
method. This led to cynicism rates ranging from 20% to 62% for the various
methods used. WEB-oriented methods showed cynicism rates ranging from
approximately 28-42%. Ironically, while brief stories (using speech,
PowerPoint text and animation) generated the least amount of cynicism (20%),
an audio story by a classic story teller generated the most cynicism (62%).
A psychodynamic model incorporating information-intake styles,
information-expression styles, and demographics was constructed to examine
the determinants of such cynicism via Logistic Regression analyses. The
model was reliable for six of the 11 variables; and numerous predictor
variables revealed the complex interplay between pedagogical technique and
the type of student. Even with popular techniques like sound-bites,
PowerPoint, animation, MPEG, stories, and the use of the Internet, there was
a substantial rate of cynicism. However, in the spirit of
"multiple-perspective-taking" these cynicism rates may be viewed as a
positive phenomenon as well as a negative phenomenon.
Media-Embedded/Embedded-Media (MEEM)
Pedagogical Techniques and Student Cynicism
The search for pedagogical techniques that will facilitate more effective
teaching is a preoccupation of most teachers. The scope of such a
search-and-advocacy endeavour is broad, ranging from top-down "direct
revelation" of codes (e.g., Moses as legislator, teacher as disseminator) to
bottom-up "discovery" (e.g., the hidden codes of Microsoft with Gates as
facilitator). Vehicles like revelation, story, dialogue, demonstration,
informing, apprenticing, constructing, and "surfing," are found in the
educational landscape competing for the teacher’s attention. Thus, we hear
expressions like, "What works for you?" "Best Practices!" "What do the
‘Experts’ do?" "I tried this…"
Some experts tell stories, and stories are, indeed, a perennial and
popular pedagogical technique—and apparently more so with the ascending
postmodern emphasis on narrative and metanarrative. Even quasi-narrative
(i.e., little stories) can be "big" techniques in education because they are
effective, nuanced, communication vehicles—they inform and entertain. People
are motivated to listen to stories. You hear the little story, the true
story, of the child who is asked, "What is the thing to do if you see thick
smoke coming from your neighbour’s house?" You smile when the child
responds, "Call the camera crew from eye witness news" (Flumen & Flumen,
1979, p.83). You smile, but you learned something about our culture and the
child’s culture. The story gets the student thinking, and the teacher
thinking, about media. Media occupy a high place in our hierarchical
cognitive networks. We may be close to the time when Maslow’s hierarchy of
needs will need revision to include media as one of the lower level needs.
Some experts see many academic disciplines as forms of storytelling.
Postman (1992), for example, sees the work of people like Marx, Weber,
Mumford, Jung, Mead, and so on, as storytelling. We learn from their
stories. Yet, not everyone agrees that stories are a good pedagogical
technique; some would make the case that stories waste time. It is possible
that one could communicate in a ten-minute lecture what a story communicates
in 100 minutes? So efficiency experts, for example, might challenge the use
of story. It is this efficiency mantra that harmonizes best with technology,
and invites technique.
Technology, then, becomes the other dominant pedagogical player. It is
not necessarily oppositional to "story’ but it is of a different genre. Such
items as alphabets, parchment, pencils, printing presses, overhead
projectors, audio tapes, radio, TV, and video, have a history presaging
educational revolution, in their respective generations. Each time,
improving efficiency is an apparent objective and outcome. More recently, it
is computers and/or the Internet holding this coveted spot as "the"
important pedagogical technique of the day. Advocates like Papert (1980,
1993), Negroponte (1995) and Turkle (1995) make the fascinating case for
technology. The more the better! In fact, Papert (1993) uses a biological or
evolutionary metaphor when he argues for more and varied technology in the
mix, "It is only in such an ecology of mutations and hybridizations of ways
of learning that a truly new mathetic culture could emerge" (p.217). Reading
such advocates fires the imagination, no doubt, of some! As with stories,
though, there are the critics (e.g., Ellul, 1964; Postman, 1992), the
concerned (Armstrong & Casement, 1998), and the contrarians (Stoll, 1999).
The critiques are credible, but generally muted in the educational milieu.
So there are two sides of each approach—pros and cons whether the art of
storytelling (ancient or modern) or the reign of technology (ancient or
modern) is the preferred road. The teacher selects. One question, an
empirical question, that needs to be addressed for the teacher is: How do
students react to these various pedagogical techniques? We can say
"everybody loves a story!" But do they? Stories have long been viewed as
both entertainment and the great teaching vehicles from our past? They are
natural motivators. Children who wanted to read stories wanted to learn to
read. Traditions are handed down through stories. Morality is encouraged by
the stories of our heroes. Stories make us laugh and weep, cringe and reach.
We are enthralled as the wordsmiths craft things of beauty, and joys
forever. Surely all students would respond favorably to stories. But do
they?
Similarly, others argue for technology as the premier teaching vehicle.
The technology provides a tool for accessing facts, databases, systematic
information, people, resources and so on. The technology provides
multi-sensory input, allows for personal control and provides a real
independence. The learners are in a position to season their knowledge plate
with the right amounts of text and image—the right amounts for them. Surely
all students would respond favorably to technology. But do they?
The extent of cynicism within a student population is one focus of this
study. Thus, there is no theoretical critique offered at this point, nor an
argument in favour of a particular technique. Rather it is an examination of
resident attitudes in a group of university students—attitudes concerning
various techniques to which they are exposed.
The particular techniques to be considered are media-based. They are
divided in to four general categories (see Table 1), with several techniques
in each category.
Table 1: Pedagogical Methods in a Media-Friendly Classroom
Pedagogical
Category |
Pedagogical
Vehicle |
Pedagogical Intent |
Technological Interfaces |
Message |
Pre-Class Activities |
Music,
PowerPoint |
Entertainment, Information,
Discovery |
Music, MP3’s, PowerPoint |
"Find the Message" |
Sound-Bite Activities |
Little Stories,
Projects,
Nano-lessons,
Gimmicks |
Information,
Constructivist |
Speech, PowerPoint, VCR,
MPEG, Drama, Artifacts, etc. |
"Build Your Message" |
WEB Activities |
Outline,
Tour,
Resources |
Information,
Technological |
Speech, Photocopies,
PowerPoint, WEB-pages, WEB-sites, WEB-Links, Video, |
"Medium is Message" |
Traditional Activities |
Research Review,
A Story |
Information,
Literate |
PowerPoint, Video, Oral,
Audio Tape |
"Message in Story" |
What kinds of reactions would students make to these various teaching
vehicles? Would one approach be preferable? Moreover, if there are different
preferences what would be the characteristics of those who prefer one
approach over another?
To examine this question approximately 500 graduate level students rated
a variety of pedagogical techniques to which they were exposed. In addition,
they were asked to provide information related to preferred intake of
information (e.g., concert-going, TV, reading fiction, etc.) as well as
preferred output qualities (as reflected in career interests). Together with
demographic information (e.g., age major, gender, etc.) these data allowed
for consideration of the determinants of interest in various pedagogical
strategies.
Method
Subjects
The subjects for this study were drawn from a population of approximately
760 students, all with at least one undergraduate degree, who were taking an
additional year of study to acquire teacher certification and the B.Ed.
degree. The responses of 506 students (of the 760 students) led to a return
rate of 67%. The characteristics of the sample may be seen in Tables 2-4.
Table 2: Gender Distribution of Sample
Gender |
% |
Non-Identified |
5.1% |
Male |
26.3% |
Female |
68.6% |
Table 3: Age Distribution of Sample
Age |
% |
Non-Identified |
3.2% |
20-24 |
44.1% |
25-29 |
34.4% |
30-34 |
7.7% |
35-39 |
5.5% |
40+ |
5.1% |
Table 4: Distribution of Majors in the Sample
Educational Background |
% |
Non-Identified Majors |
9.6% |
Performing Arts Majors |
6.8% |
Socio-type Majors |
11.4% |
Language Majors |
5.0% |
Business Majors |
3.2% |
Politico-type Majors |
3.4% |
Psych-type Majors |
14.4% |
History Majors |
6.4% |
English Majors |
7.6% |
Criminology Majors |
2.0% |
Science Majors |
10.6% |
Kinetics Majors |
7.8% |
Mass Communications Majors |
1.8% |
Geography Majors |
4.0% |
Math Majors |
2.0% |
General/Misc Majors |
4.2% |
Instruments
The instrument to collect the data involved four sections: (1) rating
(using a 5-point Likert-type scale) 11 pedagogical strategies/activities
(embedded within media, or containing embedded media), to which they had
been exposed during one two-hour class (see Table 5 for descriptions), (2)
rating on a 5-point scale 17 behavioural activities (e.g., watching drama on
TV, reading current fiction, reading the newspaper, etc.), (3) rating on a
5-point scale18 career paths (e.g., drafting, music, accounting, chemistry,
etc.) in terms of appeal, and (4) demographic information (e.g., age, major,
gender, time-of-day).
Table 5: Pedagogical Techniques During a Two-Hour Class
Technique |
Description |
Music (PRE-CLASS) |
This is a pre-class activity where music is playing prior
to the start of class. It is intended to present a relaxed and welcoming
environment. But also, the content of the music is thematically related
to the theme of the lecture. (10 minutes) |
PPT –PowerPoint
(PRE-CLASS) |
This is a pre-class activity where a PowerPoint
presentation is playing prior to the start of class. It is intended to
present interesting, often humorous, information to generate relevant
schema and thought. The content is thematically related to the theme of
the lecture. (parallels the music for about 10 minutes) |
Little Stories
(SOUND-BITES) |
During the
lecture "little stories" are shared (using PowerPoint—text, graphics,
cartoons, animation, audio) with the class. The stories relate to
personal experiences of self and others (students and teachers
previously in the class). (15 minutes) |
Sample Projects
(SOUND-BITES) |
Samples of
previous student projects are shared with the class. These are videos
(about 5 minutes in length) which are humorous, informative and
illustrative of the technological approach to doing class projects. (15
minutes) |
Nano-lessons
(SOUND-BITES) |
These are brief
‘lessons’ that could potentially equip the student with strategies to
help them in the classroom. For example, "Save Your Voice" and use light
signals or sound signals to get attention. Or, "Walk Slowly" to deal
with discipline problems. The slow pace is intimidating and it will give
you time to think about what you are going to do when you reach the
source of the problem. (10 minutes) |
Gimmicks
(SOUND-BITES) |
This involves
gimmicky techniques to get attention (a pink bicycle horn) or to get
people talking (a fluff ball). (2 minutes) |
WEB Outline (WEB) |
The syllabus for the course is provided in a printed
format, and then displayed on-line so that the hyperlinks to class
notes, assignments, and so on, may be demonstrated. (5 minutes) |
WEB Tour (WEB) |
In addition to the WEB Outline, other WEB pages are
viewed. There are pages for notification of cancelled classes, pages for
"reminders," "announcements," "updates," "class notes," and so on. (5-10
minutes) |
WEB Resources (WEB) |
On-line resources related to the lecture topic are shown,
briefly. These relate to both informational and practical resources.
(3-5 minutes) |
The Big Story
(TRADITIONAL) |
This is an audio
story that relates to the lecture theme-a story told by a classic story
teller (W.O. Mitchell). (25 minutes) |
Research
(TRADITIONAL) |
This involves a
PowerPoint presentation of several research studies related to the
lecture theme. These are empirical studies over a period of time making
an interesting educational point with applications for the teacher. (25
minutes) |
Procedure
A media-friendly classroom was used to collect the data. The classroom
seats approximately 280 students. It utilizes a large media projector, a
fixed computer, a laptop computer (both with direct connections to the
Internet), a VCR, an overhead projector, an opaque projector, an integrated
sound system, and all of these work seamlessly from a front-and-center
control panel. One week following the first class of the year the students
were asked to rate the pedagogical activities of the previous week and
report on their learning preferences, career interests and personal
information. The learning preferences were subjected to a factor analysis
using an eigenvalue of 1, varimax rotation, and a loading criterion of .4,
with at least two items loading on a factor. This revealed five factors
which were termed (1) "Literate-Types," accounting for 21.6% of the
variance, (2) "TV-Types," accounting for 13.98% of the variance, (3)
"Cultural-Performance-Types," accounting for 8.5% of the variance, (4)
"News-Types," accounting for 7.8% of the variance, and (5) "Fiction-Types,"
accounting for 6.9% of the variance (see Table 6).
Table 6: Information Intake Types
Type |
Sample Items |
Input
Characteristics |
"Literate-Types" |
Prefer biography, history,
nonfiction |
Non-fiction |
"TV-Types" |
Prefer TV dramas, movies,
sitcoms |
TV/Video |
"Cultural-Performance-Types" |
Prefer theatre, concerts, radio
music, TV music |
Performances |
"News-Types" |
Prefer TV News, Newspapers,
Newsmagazines |
News Sources |
"Fiction-Types" |
Prefer current fiction, classic
fiction |
Novels |
Next, the career preferences were subjected to a factor analysis using an
eigenvalue of 1, varimax rotation, and a loading criterion of .4, with at
least two items loading on a factor. This revealed five factors which were
termed (1) "Business-Types," accounting for 28.1% of the variance, (2)
"Tech-Types," accounting for 12.36% of the variance, (3) "Science-Types,"
accounting for 10.16% of the variance, (4) "Psych-Types," accounting for
6.77% of the variance, and (5) "Arts-Types," accounting for 5.78% of the
variance (see Table 7).
Table 7: Information Expression Types
Type |
Sample Items |
Output
Characteristics |
"Business-Types" |
Preference for banking, real
estate, hotel, retail, etc |
Negotiate, interact,
manipulate, convince… |
"Tech-Types" |
Preference for engineering,
drafting, architecture, etc |
Draw, diagram, build, create, |
"Science-Types" |
Preference for biology,
chemistry, nursing |
Experiment, classify, report,
search, collect, quantify,… |
"Psych-Types" |
Preference for criminology,
psychology, education |
Help, socialize, share, repair… |
"Arts-Types" |
Preference for music, drama |
Perform, entertain, use
costume, dramatize, write poetry, create art… |
These Information-Intake Types and the Information-Expression Types
permitted the construction of a model (see Figure 1) to predict cynical
attitudes, and allowed for a fined-grained analysis of determinants of
cynicism.
A
Learner-Processing Model Using Information-Intake Style, Demographics,
and Information-Expression Style To Explore Determinants of Pedagogical
Cynicism |
Intake Styles |
Demographics |
Expression Styles |
Literate-Types |
Major |
Business-Types |
TV-Types |
Gender |
Technical-Types |
Performance-Types |
Time-of-Day |
Science-Types |
News-Types |
Age |
Psychology-Types |
Fiction-Types |
|
Arts-Types |
Figure 1. |
|
|
The model involves a "Psychodynamic Approach" to Information Behaviour (Nahl,
2001). In effect, a personality structure is considered as the location for
information behaviour and cynicism would be related to (1) affect (negative:
uncertainty, confusion, doubt; positive: confidence, self-efficacy, and so
on), and (2) cognitive processing preferences (preferential intake styles,
and expression or output styles). The model presented in Figure 1 addresses
the cognitive components, primarily. The cynicism could be viewed as the
affective component. Of interest in this study is the relationship between
the two.
Results
Cynicism Rates
To obtain a representation of the attitudes towards the various pedagogical
techniques the ratings were collapsed into two categories Non-Cynical and
Cynical. The percentages or respondents who were Cynical within each of the
11 techniques was taken as the cynicism measure and then graphed for
comparison purposes (see Figure 2).
As may be seen in the figure the least amount of cynicism (approximately
20% of respondents) was expressed with respect to the "Little Stories."
Ironically, the most cynicism was expressed with respect to the "Big Story"
(approximately 62%). This contrast was a striking aspect of the figure.
Techniques that were getting cynical responses from about 40% of the group
were the "Nano-lessons," the "WEB Resources," and the "Gimmicks." Techniques
that were getting cynical responses from about 30% of the group were the
"WEB Outline," the "WEB Tour" and the PowerPoint Presentation of "Research."
The technology, generally, rated better than the gimmicks and Nano-lessons,
which is a plus for technology. However, the "Little Stories" received the
most indicating that technology is not the winner in this race. Remember
though, the stories were told with technology—they were embedded in
technology and they had technology embedded within them. The mix may make
the strongest format.
On the one hand, it is surprising that no one technique appealed to more
than 80% of the group, at best. Though these techniques rely heavily on the
technology advocated to make learning more palatable to today’s "media
generation," there was still a substantial cynicism rate. Moreover, this was
the first class in the students’ program—a time when optimism would be
expected to be at its peak. Yet the cynicism rates ranged from 20% to 62%.
Cynicism Determinants
To explore these cynicism rates further the available data were
configured in terms of a model involving information-intake style,
demographics, and information-expression style (see Figure 1) and then
examined using logistic regression analyses. In these analyses the five
input variables, the four demographic variables, and the five output
variables were utilized as the model to distinguish between those who liked
a particular pedagogical technique as opposed to those who did not.
Pre-Class Activities
Music. A test of the full model (the 14 predictor variables)
against a constant-only model was not reliable,
Χ2(28) = 37.31, p > .05, indicating that the
full model did not predict pre-class music-liking. Moreover, an examination
of the Wald criterion showed that none of the variables reliably predicted
music-liking.
PowerPoint. A test of the full model against a constant-only model
was not reliable, Χ2(28)
= 37.91, p > .05, indicating that the full model did not predict
pre-class PowerPoint liking. An examination of the Wald criterion showed
that Gender (Wald = 10.65, p < .01; Odds Ratio = .368) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from males), and Fiction-Types (Wald = 3.88, p <
.05; Odds Ratio = .757) was a reliable predictor (poorer rating from those
liking fiction).
Sound-Bite Activities
Little-Stories. A test of the full model against a constant-only
model was reliable, Χ2(28)
= 53.94, p < .01, indicating that the full model did distinguish
between those liking Little-Stories and those who didn’t. The variance
accounted for was small (Nagelkerke’s R Square = .175). Prediction success
for classification showed an overall success rate of 82.1%. An examination
of the Wald criterion showed that Gender (Wald = 10.61, p < .01; Odds Ratio
= .374) was a reliable predictor (better rating from females).
Sample Projects. Here also a test of the full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 48.49, p
< .01, indicating that the full model did distinguish between those liking
Project Samples and those who didn’t (variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R
Square = .155). Prediction success for classification showed an overall
success rate of 80.6%. The Wald criterion showed that Gender (Wald = 6.21, p
< .01; Odds Ratio = .467) was a reliable predictor (better rating from
females), Time (Wald = 4.65, p < .05; Odds Ratio = .793) was a reliable
predictor (better rating earlier in the day), and Kinesiology Major (Wald =
5.16, p < .05; Odds Ratio = .17) was a reliable predictor (poorer rating
from Kinesiology Majors).
Nano-lessons. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 49.07, p
< .01, distinguishing between those liking Nano-lessons and those who didn’t
(variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .14). Prediction success
for classification showed an overall success rate of 67.6%. The Wald
criterion showed that Science-Types (Wald = 4.88, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
1.297) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Science-Types), Time (Wald
= 3.89, p < .05; Odds Ratio = .842) was a reliable predictor (better rating
earlier in the day), and Arts-Types (Wald = 5.74, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
1.33) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Arts-Types).
Gimmicks. The full model was not reliable,
Χ2(28) = 29.41, p
> .05, and therefore, did not distinguish between those liking Gimmicks and
those who didn’t. An examination of the Wald criterion showed that
Science-Types (Wald = 4.37, p < .01; Odds Ratio = 1.276) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from Science-Types).
WEB Activities
WEB Outline. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 43.35, p
< .05 (variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .127). Prediction
success for classification showed an overall success rate of 72%. The Wald
criterion showed that Technical-Types (Wald = 3.7, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
1.27) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Technical-Types), Time (Wald
= 4.43, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 1.22) was a reliable predictor (better rating
earlier in the day), and TV-Types (Wald = 15.23, p < .001; Odds Ratio = 1.6)
was a reliable predictor (better rating from TV-Types).
WEB Tour. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 47.79, p < .01 (variance accounted
for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .147). Prediction success for classification
showed an overall success rate of 74.9%. The Wald criterion showed that
Psychology-Types (Wald = 10.33, p < .001; Odds Ratio = 1.63) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from Psychology-Types), and News-Types (Wald =
7.93, p < .01; Odds Ratio = 1.39) was a reliable predictor (better rating
from News-Types).
WEB Resources. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 42.65, p
< .05 (variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .119). Prediction
success for classification revealed an overall success rate of 64%. An
examination of the Wald criterion showed that Majors was relevant where
Performing-Arts Majors (Wald = 5.84, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 4.36) was a
reliable predictor (better rating from Performing-Arts-Types), History
Majors (Wald = 7.38, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 5.71) was a reliable predictor
(better rating from History Majors), Criminology Majors (Wald = 6.15, p <
.05; Odds Ratio = 18.34) was a reliable predictor (better rating from
Criminology Majors), Science Majors (Wald = 5.06, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
3.64) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Science Majors), and
News-Types (Wald = 4.28, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 1.24) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from News-Types).
Traditional Activities
Research Review. A test of the full model against a constant-only
model was not reliable, Χ2(28)
= 34.13, p > .05. The Wald criterion showed that History Majors (Wald
= 4.25, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 4.42) was a reliable predictor (better rating
from History Majors), and Science Majors (Wald = 5.32, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
4.32) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Science Majors).
The "Big" Story. The full model was not reliable,
Χ2(28) = 29.44, p
> .05. The Wald criterion showed that Literate-Types (Wald = 4.45, p < .05;
Odds Ratio = 1.25) was a reliable predictor (better rating from
Literate-Types), and Fiction-Types (Wald = 4.55, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 1.27)
was a reliable predictor (better rating from Fiction-Types).
Discussion
The most striking aspect of these data is a clear and substantial
cynicism rate for a broad range of pedagogical techniques. Even the
stories—under the category of Sound-Bites—were viewed in less than positive
ways by some (about 20%). This occurred regardless of the optimal conditions
associated with the "first class" of the semester, a time when optimism
should be high. On the positive side, the least amount of cynicism was
expressed with respect to the "Little Stories," and therefore, "story,"
rather than "technology," would seem to be the more appealing technique for
students. But ironically, and more striking, was the cynicism expressed with
respect to a classic story teller like W.O. Mitchell. The cynicism rate for
this story was 62%. Thus, stories may capture both the high and the low of
the cynicism responses. One difference between the two stories was the time
allotment (a few minutes versus 25 minutes) which may indicate that a short
auditory attention span for today’s adult students is the source of the
differenced.
The WEB components also revealed substantial cynicism (28% to 42%).
Technological and media-friendly techniques may be appealing to the
majority, but it would seem that a large portion of the student population
would be somewhat averse to this format. To one who likes media this is both
surprising and disappointing. Apparently, one cannot rely, or should not
rely, on the technological media even if embedded within stories, or used as
vehicles for stories.
Fluctuations were also evident within the WEB techniques: the WEB-Outline
and the WEB-Tour were viewed more favourably than the WEB-Resources. This
difference is likely related to the personal and immediate implications of
the WEB-Outline and the WEB-Tour. These were directly relevant for course
requirements, whereas, the WEB-Resources were not immediately relevant.
Generally, the pedagogical hope offered by media needs to be tempered by the
normal cynicism rates in any student population, as well as such variables
as time, relevance, and as may be seem below, the demographic and
psychodynamics qualities of the learner.
The model configured to try and get a better understanding of some of the
determinants of these attitudes was informative. This Input-Output
Psychodynamic Model of Learner-Processing revealed numerous potential
determinants of cynical attitudes. The model was viewed as preliminary,
exploratory, and a broad ranging configuration of learner-processing, yet it
did allow for two levels of analysis and commentary here.
On the one hand, the model was reliable for distinguishing Cynics from
Non-Cynics for Sound-Bite Techniques (Little Stories, Project Samples, and
Nano-lessons) and WEB Techniques (Outline, Tour, Resources). It was not
reliable with respect to the Pre-class Activities or the Traditional
Activities. When reliable, however, the amount of variance explained was not
large (11.9% to 17.5%), so a tempered response, at best, is warranted.
On the other hand, the specific variables that were reliable predictors
are of interest in understanding why there would be differential responses
to the pedagogical techniques. This is theoretically interesting, and may
lead to better models of learning and better models of teaching. Thus, there
is a rationale for considering the responses in terms of Demographics, Input
variables and Output variables. That interesting differences exist is not to
argue that instructors should try to match pedagogical technique with the
learning style of the students; this could be a logistical nightmare.
Rather, on the practical side, understanding learner differences could
inform instructors about the need for varied methods, could suggest ‘better’
methods, and could create an awareness of the seemingly problematic methods.
Learner Differences: Demographics
In terms of demographics, Gender was a reliable predictor for: (1) the
Pre-Class PowerPoint (males were more likely to like it), (2) the Little
Stories (females liked those), (3) the Sample Projects (females liked
those). So males may gravitate to the visual and the technological, while
females are more oriented to story (the story of the "Sample Projects," and
the "Little Stories"). This seems to fit our stereotypes.
Time-of-Day was a reliable predictor with more favourable ratings earlier
in the day on (1) Nano-lessons, (2) Sample Projects, and (3) the WEB
Outline. There seems to have been an attitudinal shift later in the day.
What these three techniques may have in common is their function of
providing specific information on behavioural demands. If students are more
cynical about these later in the day it may be because they are using more
cognitively sophisticated processing later in the day. There is a body of
literature suggesting enhanced learning later in the day for certain types
of tasks (Baddeley, Hatter, Scott & Snashall, Blake, 1967; Folkard, 1979:
Folkard & Monk, 1978; Morton, 1986; Morton & Kershner, 1985, 1991; Tilley &
Warren, 1983). A more critical attitude, or cynicism, later in the day, is
consistent with this literature.
Major was a reliable predictor for Sample Projects (with a poorer rating
from Kinesiology Majors). Major was also a reliable predictor for Research
(with better ratings—less cynicism—from History Majors and Science Majors).
The higher "Research" ratings for History and Science Majors make sense.
These majors would be heavily invested in research activity and thus see
value in research. The poorer rating from Kinesiology Majors may be
logically linked to the action style of this group. Preferring activity they
may find some projects too sedentary, or too formal. With respect to the
WEB-Resources, the following Majors were reliable predictors: Performing
Arts, History, Criminology, and Science (each showing more favourable
attitudes). This reveals an interesting and diverse range of background
variables that seem to impact the valuing of WEB resources. While History,
Criminology and Science majors would logically be drawn to such resources,
it is not clear what is drawing the Performing Arts majors.
Learner Characteristics: Input Variables
Fiction-Types was a reliable predictor of the cynicism towards the
Pre-Class PowerPoint. They didn’t care for it. But they did like the "Big
Story," as did those scoring higher on the Literate-Types scale. This makes
sense. Those who like literature especially should be drawn to the sense of
story—particularly with a classic storyteller like W.O. Mitchell.
TV-Types predicted WEB-Outline cynicism rate, while News-Types predicted
WEB-Tour cynicism rate. That students who like the TV (TV-Types) and news
sources (News-Types) would also appreciate the WEB is reasonable. A computer
hooked up to the WEB is somewhat homologous with the TV and News sources.
Indeed, such a tour could be construed as a form of news media, and similar
to watching TV.
Learner Characteristics: Output Variables
Here a number of reliable predictors of cynicism were evident. The
Science-Types scale predicted the Nano-lessons cynicism rate, and the
Gimmicks cynicism rate. Science-Types liked these sound-bites (the
Nano-lessons and the Gimmicks). While these do not map onto the output
characteristics mentioned in Table 7 earlier, they do have an associated
quality—clear, terse action. This might appeal to "Science-Types."
The Arts-Types scale also predicted the Nano-lessons cynicism rate. One
could speculate that the Arts-Types liked the Nano-lessons because they are
short and dramatic, and at times have performance elements attached to them.
The Technical-Types scale predicted the WEB-Outline cynicism rate. They
liked it. Technical-Types may have a preference for design, structure,
technique, and technology.
The Psychology-Types scale predicted the WEB-Tour cynicism rate. Perhaps
the Psychology-Types liked the WEB Tour because it provided information they
could use in their own outreach. In addition, it was an educational
psychology class so the information toured would align with their interests.
Two things are evident here: (1) an apparently complex interplay between
the pedagogical technique one experiences, one’s preferred modes of
information intake, one’s background, and one’s perceived or preferential
output style, and (2) a cynicism with respect to various pedagogical
techniques that is influenced by this complex interplay. Even highly rated,
and highly valued, techniques have more than their fair share of detractors,
and the detractors are interesting in unique ways.
The Positive Twist
Such information on cynicism may be of value to teachers. Some may wish
to utilize techniques associated with lower rates of cynicism. Others may
wish to try and match learners and techniques. However, these are not the
recommendations being made here. There is a more interesting positive twist
that emerges from these findings.
Such information may be of value to administrators. Knowing that there is
a residual cynicism rate in a body of university students may temper
judgments made of faculty members (regarding renewal, promotion and tenure)
which are based on student ratings. But this is not the positive twist
either.
The positive twist is the argument that such cynicism is valuable. It is
encouraging, and it should be encouraged, cultivated, nurtured and extended.
Teachers, perhaps, should be designing their courses to generate more
cynicism, not less. The rationale for this argument emerges from (1)
empirical studies (e.g., Milgram, 1974), (2) informants (e.g., Garfinkle,
2000; Foucault, 1977; Turkle, 1995), and (3) the critics of
technology/technique (e.g., Ellul, 1981; Postman, 1992). With respect to
empirical studies, consider Milgram’s famous experiments where subjects were
requested to administer electric shock to a person (an accomplice of the
experimenter) in response to directives from the authority figure (the
researcher). Even when the person receiving the shock appeared to be under
great duress many of the subjects continued to administer shock in response
to the authority figure’s request. About 35% refused—echoing an interesting
rate of cynicism, the cynicism with respect to pedagogy.
With respect to informants, the implication of the surveillance we are
exposed to as a function of technology alarms us. We are at risk, and the
cynics and critics are a key defence. Thus, when Foucault informs us of the
control issues in prisons (and other institutions) as a result of
surveillance, we become more cynical. When Turkle (1995) informs us of what
is actually happening on the WEB, we become more cynical. When Garfinkle
alerts us to the war on privacy by government, business, and neighbours, we
call for more cynicism. We see that there are cynics operating in the
community and wonder how we can encourage more cynics.
The critic, Ellul, argues that educators need to be teaching students to
live "in" technology/technique, but "against" technology/technique. He sees
real danger. Whereas some (see Postman, 1992) at least note that the
computer is the metaphor of our age—the paradigmatic metaphor—Ellul has a
broader vision; for him technology/technique is more than metaphor. The
place occupied by "capital" for the past 200 years has now been supplanted
by "technology/technique," as Ellul sees it. Power, therefore, has a new
substrate; it has shifted to "technology/technique." In addition to this
profound social and political effect, there are epistemological effects; he
contends that technology/technique has two major epistemological effects—it
suppresses the subject, and it suppresses meaning. "The means has entirely
replaced the meaning (Ellul, 1980, p.254). Is this what McLuhan meant when
contending that the "medium is the message?" The means becomes the end. If
he is right, and he makes a good case for this, it is the cynics who will
help restore and strengthen the subject, and recover meaning in the presence
of an ever widening exercise of "technology/technique." If he is right such
cynicism should be cultivated—perhaps developing the self-appointed critic,
the cross-examiner, "the devil’s advocate," the Socratic gadfly, the wearer
of deBono’s "Black Hat," the contrarian, and so on. The call is out.
References
Armstrong, A. & Casement, C. (1998). The child and the machine. Toronto:
Key Porter Books.
Baddeley, A. D., Hatter, J. E., Scott, D., & Snashall, A. (1970). Memory
and time of day. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22,
605-609.
Blake, M. J. F. (1967). Time of day effects on performance in a range of
tasks. Psychonomic Science, 9, 349-350.
Ellul, J. (1964). The technological society. New York: Vintage
Books.
Ellul, J. (1980). The technological system. New York: Continuum.
Ellul, J. (1981). Perspectives on our age, Jacques Ellul speaks on his
life and work. Editor W. H. Vanderburg. Toronto: House of Anansi Press
Ltd.
Flumen, A. F. & Flumen, L. B. (1979). WISCOS and WPPSICLES. Journal of
School Psychology, 17, 82-85.
Folkard, S. (1979). Time of day and level of processing. Memory and
Cognition, 7, 247-252.
Folkard, S. & Monk, H. (1978). Time of day effects in immediate and
delayed memory. In M. M. Gruenberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.),
Practical aspects of memory. London: Academic Press.
Foucault. M. (1977). Discipline and punish: The birth of the prison.
New York: Pantheon.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view.
New York: Harper & Row.
Morton, L. L. (1986). A single-subject study of the effects of time on
task and time of day on productivity and achievement in a dysgraphic student
,. Canadian Journal for Exceptional Children, 3, 23-28.
Morton, L. L. & Kershner, J. R. (1985). Time-of-day effects upon
children's memory and analogical reasoning. The Alberta Journal of
Educational Research, 31, 26-34.
Morton, L. L. & Kershner, J. R. (1991). Time-of-day effects on
neuropsychological behaviors as measured by dichotic listening . The
International Journal of Neuroscience, 59, 241-251.
Nahl. D. (2001). A conceptual framework for explaining information
behavior. SIMILE 1. Retrieved November 30th, 2001 from
http://www.utpjournals.com/jour.ihtml?lp=simile/issue2/issue2toc.html
Negroponte, N. (1995). Being digital. New York: Vintage
Books.
Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms. New York: Basic Books.
Papert, S. (1993). The children’s machine. New York: Basic Books.
Postman, N. (1992). Technopoly. New York: Vintage Books.
Stoll. C. (1999). High tech heretic. New York: Doubleday.
Tilley, A. & Warren, P. (1983). Retrieval from semantic memory at
different times of day. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 9, 718-724.
Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the screen. New York: Touchstone.
|
|
Appendix
Feedback in Educational Psychology (Correlates for Instructional Formats)
How would you rate the
following instructional formats used in week 1 in terms of your liking
or disliking? |
Poor
1 |
2 |
So/So
3 |
4 |
Great
5 |
Pre-class music |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Pre-class Powerpoint presentation on
different types of teachers. |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Class references to other student
experiences. |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Class references to other student video
projects. |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Nano-lessons |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
The WEB Based Course Outline |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Tour of the Class WEB Site |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
The Audio story on Summer Vacation. |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
WEB Resources You can use for Dealing with
Summer Vacations |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Presentation on Research on Summer Vacation
Effects |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Gimmicks: The Pink Horn, The Fluff Ball |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
How would you rate your
behaviour/activities with respect to the following: |
Very Little
1 |
2 |
So/So
3 |
4 |
A Lot
5 |
Watching the News on TV |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Watching Sports on TV |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Watching Sitcoms on TV |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Watching Drama on TV |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Watching TV Movies |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Watching MuchMusic |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Listening to Music Radio |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Listening to Talk Radio |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Attending Theatre (Stratford, Shaw, Little
Theatre) |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Attending Concerts (Band, Orchestra, Rock,
etc.) |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
How would you rate your
behaviour/activities with respect to the following: |
Very Little
1 |
2 |
So/So
3 |
4 |
A Lot
5 |
Reading the Local Newspaper |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Reading Newsmagazines (Time, Newsweek,
Vanity Fair, Oprah, etc.) |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Reading current fiction |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Reading classic fiction… |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Reading biography… |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Reading history… |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Reading other non-fiction… |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
How would you rate the
following as a career path that would appeal to you? |
No Appeal
1 |
2 |
So/So
3 |
4 |
Much Appeal
5 |
Engineering |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Drafting |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Technology |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Biology |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Chemistry |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Criminology |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Music |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Drama |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Architecture |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Nursing |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Education |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Psychology |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Real estate |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Retailing |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Hotel services |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Accounting |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Business |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Banking |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
Demographic Information |
Circle or write response |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Gender? |
Male |
Female |
Other |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Age? |
20-24 |
25-29 |
30-34 |
35-39 |
40+ |
|
|
Undergraduate Major? |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Your Preferred Learning Style? |
Listening |
Reading |
Doing |
Seeing |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Class Section |
Mon@8am |
Mon@1pm |
Mon@5pm |
Tues@3pm |
Wed@8am |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Debriefing |
Issue |
|
Language: |
- Nice to
see people remembered “Larry’s Mother” Test (indeed, text
was dense at times).
- Need for
Precise Definitions especially with the key term “cynicism.” Many
identified this as a problem, or at least questionable.
|
It is best to
avoid a term that lacks sufficient warrant (psychometric warrant,
linguistic warrant, or theoretical warrant). So if neither
“cynicism” nor “critical thinking” have sufficient warrant what term
would be acceptable here? What we really have is something like
“positive regard,” “favoured,” “liking,” and so on, on the positive
end of the continuum, and their opposites on the other end. . |
Pet peeve of
mine—lack of clarity. A major criticism of postmodernism is the way
postmoderns co-opt the language, twist it, and obfuscate such that
dialogue is compromised (it leads to propaganda, not discovery…). |
Yet
the concept under investigation does have elements of cynicism.
Further research might help to make the case that this is truly
“cynicism” but for the current context the challenge is a good one. |
Clarity! Clarity! Clarity! |
|
Instruments
|
Levels:
- Published,
commercial, good psychometric quality (i.e., norms, reliability,
and validity), can be costly, have a good research history;
- Public
domain, with some psychometric qualities (i.e., attempts to
establish reliability and validity), a history that facilitates
comparisons, and judgments of quality
- Researcher
made, but with measures of factor structure, internal consistency,
and efforts to establish validity either empirically or
theoretically…
- Researcher
made with no psychometric support
|
My rules for
dissertations and theses (Level 1 and 2 okay, and maybe 3, but
I would discourage 3 unless it was supported by 1 or 2 as well). My
rules for projects and major papers (1-4, since less rigour is
required). |
I wouldn't let a student who wanted
to do a master's thesis use such an instrument (as was used in the
present study), unless it was part
of a battery that included more standardized instruments. |
Precision! Precision! Precision! |
|
Design |
Longitudinal?
Yes, it would be a good idea, if one thought the benefits would
outweigh the costs. (Cost/benefit analysis does come into play when
making research decisions). More data? Yes, more data, more
variables, more participants are always desirable in quantitative
research. (Again, there is the cost/benefit analysis issue). I
personally don't feel investing an additional effort at this point
is warranted. It isn't interesting enough! |
More important:
structure the research so you are testing competing theories or
competing models, or competing sets of empirical studies. The ideal
is to advance one as opposed to the other, or refute both. This
study doesn't meet such criteria. |
Loss of Data:
(I think everyone missed this one as well.). By collapsing from five
categories to two on each scale, information about degrees of
difference is lost. Necessary for Logistic Regression Analyses, but
it might be valuable to include the five-point scale data as an
appendix for those who want to take a more fine-grained look. |
Some structural elements are more important than
others! |
|
Weak Study |
Interesting
Things: You can extract interesting things even from poor studies,
and from weak instruments (typical of major papers, and project).
They can
function as a seeding process and trigger research ideas and
directions.
Can
illustrate a model.
They can lead
to changes in practice. |
There is a
case for getting even weak papers published. Lesser quality journals
(e.g., tier 2, 3 and 4 journals) often provide the researcher or
practitioner with relevant information. |
Colloquium
Format: Can be associated with weaker studies. Often a draft. Often
research that one knows won’t get by a journal editor or review
process… |
Be open to the interesting! |
|
Weak
Citations |
Some had
rightfully called for more literature in the review. I had my GA do
a search back in December to get an idea of what was really out
there. Lots, and lots! |
You missed
the evidence of weaker journals (tier 2-4) for the time-of-day data.
This doesn’t mean the findings are not reliable, or valid or
credible, only that you should be more cautious. I can illustrate
this with a personal experience (story). I submitted a paper for
publication to Perceptual & Motor Skills a number of years ago. This
was a journal I had read numerous times as they published a large
number of papers and often on the kind of esoteric topics that
interested me. There was a blind peer review process and the paper
was accepted. Shortly thereafter we received a bill for $300.00. We
missed the fine print, and just assumed that acceptance implied
receipt of 50 free copies for distribution purposes (typical of most
journals). So now I am suspicious of papers published in “Perceptual
& Motor Skills.” I don’t discount the articles but I read them more
critically. Implication: Recently, I read the book (Can we be good
without God,” by Robert Buckman, the president of the Canadian
Humanist Association, a cardiologist at the University of Toronto.
In making a particular point in his argument he cited favourably,
and numerous times, a particular researcher. When one examined the
research though one was struck by the fact that about 95% of the
publications of this researcher are in Perceptual & Motor Skills. It
doesn’t imply the data are incorrect, but it does raise a flag.
Familiarity with the journals can help you to be a more discerning
reader. |
Be discerning! Discriminating! Critical! Cynical? |
|
Reading |
People were reporting difficulty
reading the paper. Admittedly there was dense text, too broadly
focused, but more likely dense statistics were a source of
irritation. One suggestion is to continue to acquire familiarity
with statistical terms and elements of quantitative research. |
Another
suggestion relates to strategy--that is, Reading Style
Some people
may be using the typical academic reading style--a linear
strategy—trying to read everything from first to last, and in order.
It is wise to learn the structure of academic articles and then read
selectively. Perhaps just the abstract will do, perhaps skipping the
methodology and/or the results sections, will do. Perhaps just the
literature review is what you need. Or perhaps the abstract, then
the first paragraph of the discussion then the methodology section,
then the literature review, then the results, and finally the full
discussion. Perhaps, just the results will do
Selective
reading! |
Hypertexting? |
Use Strategy! |
|
|
|
|
|
The Revision |
Why Do Some Resist Technology In Classrooms?
Dr. L. L. Morton
University of Windsor
Faculty of Education
401 Sunset Avenue
Windsor, Ontario
N9B3P4
e-mail morton@uwindsor.ca
February 27, 2002
Abstract
Approximately 500 post baccalaureate students were asked to respond to 11
pedagogical techniques to which they were exposed in a media-friendly
lecture hall. At times the techniques were embedded in media, at other times
the techniques had media embedded within them. The data allowed for students
to be grouped into "non-likers" (those who showed no positive response to a
particular method) and "likers." This led to non-liking rates ranging from
20% to 62% for the various media-methods used. WEB-oriented methods showed
non-liking rates ranging from approximately 28-42%. Ironically, while brief
stories (using speech, PowerPoint text and animation) generated the least
amount of non-liking (20%), an audio story by a classic story teller
generated the most non-liking (62%). While the data collection protocol was
weak and adequate psychometric properties of the data collection instrument
were not demonstrable the findings are presented here as "quite
interesting." Moreover, it was possible to construct a psychodynamic model
incorporating information-intake styles, information-expression styles, and
demographics to examine the determinants of such liking/non-liking via
Logistic Regression analyses. The model was reliable for six of the 11
variables; and numerous predictor variables revealed the complex interplay
between pedagogical technique and the type of student. Even with popular
techniques like sound-bites, PowerPoint, animation, MPEG, stories, and the
use of the Internet, there was a substantial rate of non-liking. However, in
the spirit of "multiple-perspective-taking" these non-liking rates may be
viewed as a positive phenomenon as well as a negative phenomenon.
Why Do Some Resist Technology In Classrooms?
The search for effective pedagogical practice is a preoccupation of many
teachers. The scope of such a search-and-advocacy endeavour is broad,
ranging from effective top-down techniques (e.g., presentation, lecture,
video, and story) to constructivist bottom-up techniques (e.g., discovery,
projects, and creative endeavours). A multiplicity of vehicles like story,
dialogue, demonstration, informing, apprenticing, constructing, and
"surfing," are found in the educational landscape competing for the
teacher’s attention. Thus, we hear expressions like, "What works for you?"
"Best Practices!" "What do the ‘Experts’ do?" "I tried this…" "Students
don’t like that technique," and so on.
Some might argue that stories are perennial, premier and most popular
pedagogical technique—and apparently more so with the ascending postmodern
emphasis on narrative and metanarrative. Even quasi-narrative (i.e., little
stories) can be "big" techniques in education because they are effective,
nuanced, communication vehicles—they inform and entertain. People are
motivated to listen to stories. You hear the little story, the true story,
of the child who is asked, "What is the thing to do if you see thick smoke
coming from your neighbour’s house?" You might smile when the child
responds, "Call the camera crew from eye witness news" (Flumen & Flumen,
1979, p.83). You might smile, but you learned something about our culture
and the child’s culture. The story gets the student thinking, and the
teacher thinking, about media, ironically—the impact of media, the use of
media, and the value of media. Media occupy high places in both our
cognitive networks and our educational milieu, as does the story.
It is possible to consider many academic disciplines as forms of
storytelling. Postman (1992), for example, sees the work of people like
Marx, Weber, Mumford, Jung, Mead, and so on, as storytelling. We learn from
their stories. Yet, not everyone agrees that stories are a good pedagogical
technique; some would make the case that stories waste time. It is possible
that one could communicate in a ten-minute lecture what a story communicates
in 100 minutes? So efficiency experts, for example, might challenge the use
of story. It is this efficiency mantra that harmonizes best with technology,
and invites technique. So again, there is this ironic mix of story and
technology.
Thus, technology, or technique, becomes another dominant pedagogical
player. It is not necessarily oppositional to "story’ but it is of a
different genre. Such items as alphabets, parchment, pencils, printing
presses, overhead projectors, audio tapes, radio, TV, and video, have a
history presaging educational revolution, in their respective generations.
Each time, improving efficiency is an apparent objective and outcome. More
recently, it is computers and/or the Internet holding this coveted spot as
"the" important pedagogical technique of the day. Advocates like Papert
(1980, 1993), Negroponte (1995) and Turkle (1995) make the fascinating case
for technology. The more the better! In fact, Papert (1993) uses a
biological or evolutionary metaphor when he argues for more and varied
technology in the mix, "It is only in such an ecology of mutations and
hybridizations of ways of learning that a truly new mathetic culture could
emerge" (p.217). Thus Papert gains some supporters, and the majority of
classrooms get wired-up to the Net. As with stories, though, there are the
critics (e.g., Ellul, 1964; Postman, 1992), the concerned (Armstrong &
Casement, 1998), and the contrarians (Stoll, 1999). These credible critiques
are muted or heard as white noise in the current environment.
So there are two sides with respect to these approaches—pros and cons
whether the art of storytelling (ancient or modern) or the reign of
technology (ancient or modern) is the preferred road. One question, an
empirical question, that arises for the teacher is: How do students react to
these various pedagogical techniques? Why do some resist? We could say
"everybody loves a story!" But do they? Stories have long been viewed as
both entertainment and the great teaching vehicles from our past? They are
natural motivators. Children, wanting to read stories, are motivated to
learn to read, or so the story goes. Stories supply information. Traditions
are handed down through stories. Morality is encouraged by the stories of
our heroes. Stories make us laugh and weep, cringe and reach. We are
enthralled as the wordsmiths craft things of beauty. Surely all students
would respond favorably to stories. But do they?
Yet, others argue for technology as the premier teaching vehicle. The
technology provides a tool for accessing facts, databases, systematic
information, people, resources and so on. The technology provides
multi-sensory input, allows for personal control and provides a real
independence. The learners are in a position to season their knowledge plate
with the right amounts of text and image, substance and appetizer—the right
amounts for them. All students respond favorably to technology! But do they?
The extent of such liking/non-liking within a student population is a
focus of this present study. Thus, there is no theoretical critique offered
at this point, nor an argument in favour of a particular technique. Rather
it is an examination of resident attitudes in a group of university
students—attitudes concerning various pedagogical techniques to which they
are exposed. The particular techniques to be considered are media-based.
They are divided into four general categories (see Table 1), with several
techniques in each category.
Table 1: Pedagogical Methods in a Media-Friendly Classroom
Pedagogical Category |
Pedagogical Vehicle |
Pedagogical Intent |
Technological Interfaces |
Message |
Pre-Class Activities |
Music, PowerPoint |
Entertainment, Information, Discovery |
Music, MP3’s, PowerPoint |
"Find the Message" |
Sound-Bite Activities |
Little Stories, Projects,
Nano-lessons,
Gimmicks |
Information, Constructivist |
Speech, PowerPoint, VCR, MPEG, Drama,
Artifacts, etc. |
"Build Your Message" |
WEB Activities |
Outline, Tour,
Resources |
Information, Technological |
Speech, Photocopies, PowerPoint, WEB-pages,
WEB-sites, WEB-Links, Video, |
"Medium is Message" |
Traditional Activities |
Research Review, A Story |
Information, Literate |
PowerPoint, Video, Oral, Audio Tape |
"Message in Story" |
What kinds of reactions would students show to these various teaching
vehicles? Would one approach be more preferable? Moreover, if there are
different preferences what would be the characteristics of those who prefer
one approach over another?
The opportunity to examine this question emerged somewhat
serendipitously. Having noticed a great deal of animosity towards a
particular classroom activity in one class the situation was explored
further by having approximately 500 post-baccalaureate level students simply
give a rating for a variety of pedagogical techniques to which they were
exposed in a class. In addition, in an attempt to explore some of the
determinants of such animosity, they were asked to provide ratings related
to different information-intake formats (e.g., concert-going, TV, reading
fiction, etc.) as well as preferred output qualities (as reflected in career
interests). Together with demographic information (e.g., age major, gender,
etc.) these data allowed for at least a preliminary consideration of some of
the determinants of interest in various pedagogical strategies.
Method
Subjects
The subjects for this study were drawn from a population of approximately
760 students, all with at least one undergraduate degree, who were taking an
additional year of study to acquire teacher certification and the B.Ed.
degree. The responses of 506 students (of the 760 students) led to a return
rate of 67%. Demographic characteristics of the sample may be seen in Tables
2-4.
Table 2: Gender Distribution of Sample
Gender |
% |
Non-Identified |
5.1% |
Male |
26.3% |
Female |
68.6% |
Table 3: Age Distribution of Sample
Age |
% |
Non-Identified |
3.2% |
20-24 |
44.1% |
25-29 |
34.4% |
30-34 |
7.7% |
35-39 |
5.5% |
40+ |
5.1% |
Table 4: Distribution of Majors in the Sample
Educational Background |
% |
Non-Identified Majors |
9.6% |
Performing Arts Majors |
6.8% |
Sociology-type Majors |
11.4% |
Language Majors |
5.0% |
Business Majors |
3.2% |
Politico-type Majors |
3.4% |
Psychology-type Majors |
14.4% |
History Majors |
6.4% |
English Majors |
7.6% |
Criminology Majors |
2.0% |
Science Majors |
10.6% |
Kinetics Majors |
7.8% |
Mass Communications Majors |
1.8% |
Geography Majors |
4.0% |
Math Majors |
2.0% |
General Majors |
4.2% |
Instruments
The instrument to collect the data involved four sections: (1) rating
(using a 5-point Likert-type scale) 11 pedagogical strategies/activities
(linked to media), to which they had been exposed during one two-hour class
(see Table 5 for descriptions), (2) rating on a 5-point scale 17 behavioural
activities (e.g., watching drama on TV, reading current fiction, reading the
newspaper, etc.), (3) rating on a 5-point scale18 career paths (e.g.,
drafting, music, accounting, chemistry, etc.) in terms of appeal, and (4)
demographic information (e.g., age, major, gender, time-of-day).
Table 5: Pedagogical Techniques During a Two-Hour Class
Technique |
Description |
Music (PRE-CLASS) |
This is a pre-class activity where music is playing prior
to the start of class. It is intended to present a relaxed and welcoming
environment. But also, the content of the music is thematically related
to the theme of the lecture. (10 minutes) |
PPT –PowerPoint
(PRE-CLASS) |
This is a pre-class activity where a PowerPoint
presentation is playing prior to the start of class. It is intended to
present interesting, often humorous, information to generate relevant
schema and thought. The content is thematically related to the theme of
the lecture. (parallels the music for about 10 minutes) |
Little Stories
(SOUND-BITES) |
During the
lecture "little stories" are shared (using PowerPoint—text, graphics,
cartoons, animation, audio) with the class. The stories relate to
personal experiences of self and others (students and teachers
previously in the class). (15 minutes) |
Sample Projects
(SOUND-BITES) |
Samples of
previous student projects are shared with the class. These are videos
(about 5 minutes in length) which are humorous, informative and
illustrative of the technological approach to doing class projects. (15
minutes) |
Nano-lessons
(SOUND-BITES) |
These are brief
‘lessons’ that could potentially equip the student with strategies to
help them in the classroom. For example, "Save Your Voice" and use light
signals or sound signals to get attention. Or, "Walk Slowly" to deal
with discipline problems. The slow pace is intimidating and it will give
you time to think about what you are going to do when you reach the
source of the problem. (10 minutes) |
Gimmicks
(SOUND-BITES) |
This involves
gimmicky techniques to get attention (a pink bicycle horn) or to get
people talking (a fluff ball). (2 minutes) |
WEB Outline (WEB) |
The syllabus for the course is provided in a printed
format, and then displayed on-line so that the hyperlinks to class
notes, assignments, and so on, may be demonstrated. (5 minutes) |
WEB Tour (WEB) |
In addition to the WEB Outline, other WEB pages are
viewed. There are pages for notification of cancelled classes, pages for
"reminders," "announcements," "updates," "class notes," and so on. (5-10
minutes) |
WEB Resources (WEB) |
On-line resources related to the lecture topic are shown,
briefly. These relate to both informational and practical resources.
(3-5 minutes) |
The Big Story
(TRADITIONAL) |
This is an audio
story that relates to the lecture theme-a story told by a classic story
teller (W.O. Mitchell). (25 minutes) |
Research
(TRADITIONAL) |
This involves a
PowerPoint presentation of several research studies related to the
lecture theme. These are empirical studies over a period of time making
an interesting educational point with applications for the teacher. (25
minutes) |
Procedure
The instruction occurred in a media-friendly classroom. The classroom
seats approximately 280 students. It utilizes a large media projector, a
fixed computer, a laptop computer (both with direct connections to the
Internet), a VCR, an overhead projector, an opaque projector, an integrated
sound system, and all of these work seamlessly from a front-and-center
control panel. One week following the first class of the year the students
were asked to reflect on the pedagogical activities of the previous week and
then rate them. They were also asked to report on some information-intake
activities, career interests and personal information. While the initial
intent was to gather information that could be relevant for instructional
practices there was a suspicion that something interesting might emerge from
these data, and therefore the protocol was submitted to an ethics committee
for review, a "just-in-case" strategy.
The information-intake activities were subjected to a factor analysis
using an eigenvalue of 1, varimax rotation, and a loading criterion of .4,
with at least two items loading on a factor. This revealed five factors
which were termed (1) "Literate-Types," accounting for 21.6% of the
variance, (2) "TV-Types," accounting for 13.98% of the variance, (3)
"Cultural-Performance-Types," accounting for 8.5% of the variance, (4)
"News-Types," accounting for 7.8% of the variance, and (5) "Fiction-Types,"
accounting for 6.9% of the variance (see Table 6). The sample was split in
half and two additional factor analyses were run (one on each sample, with
the second as a confirmatory factor analysis) and the same five types
emerged in both analyses.
Table 6: Information Intake Types
Type |
Sample Items |
Input
Characteristics |
"Literate-Types" |
Prefer biography, history,
nonfiction |
Non-fiction |
"TV-Types" |
Prefer TV dramas, movies,
sitcoms |
TV/Video |
"Cultural-Performance-Types" |
Prefer theatre, concerts, radio
music, TV music |
Performances |
"News-Types" |
Prefer TV News, Newspapers,
Newsmagazines |
News Sources |
"Fiction-Types" |
Prefer current fiction, classic
fiction |
Novels |
Next, the career preferences were subjected to a factor analysis using
the same criteria as the former factor analyses. This revealed five factors
which were termed (1) "Business-Types," accounting for 28.1% of the
variance, (2) "Tech-Types," accounting for 12.36% of the variance, (3)
"Science-Types," accounting for 10.16% of the variance, (4)
"Psychology-Types," accounting for 6.77% of the variance, and (5)
"Arts-Types," accounting for 5.78% of the variance (see Table 7). Again the
sample was split in half and two additional factor analyses were run (one on
each sample, with the second as a confirmatory factor analysis) and the same
five types were evident in both analyses.
Table 7: Information Expression Types
Type |
Sample Items |
Inferred
Output-Expressive Characteristics |
"Business-Types" |
Preference for banking, real
estate, hotel, retail, etc |
Negotiate, interact,
manipulate, convince… |
"Tech-Types" |
Preference for engineering,
drafting, architecture, etc |
Draw, diagram, build, create, |
"Science-Types" |
Preference for biology,
chemistry, nursing |
Experiment, classify, report,
search, collect, quantify,… |
"Psychology-Types" |
Preference for criminology,
psychology, education |
Help, socialize, share, repair… |
"Arts-Types" |
Preference for music, drama |
Perform, entertain, use
costume, dramatize, write poetry, create art… |
These Information-Intake Types and the Information-Expression Types
permitted the construction of a model (see Figure 1) to predict the
"non-liking" attitudes, and allowed for a fined-grained analysis of
determinants of non-liking.
A
Learner-Processing Model Using Information-Intake Style, Demographics,
and Information-Expression Style To Explore Determinants of Pedagogical
Non-liking |
Intake Styles |
Demographics |
Expression Styles |
Literate-Types |
Major |
Business-Types |
TV-Types |
Gender |
Technical-Types |
Performance-Types |
Time-of-Day |
Science-Types |
News-Types |
Age |
Psychology-Types |
Fiction-Types |
|
Arts-Types |
Figure 1. |
|
|
The model involves a "Psychodynamic Approach" to Information Behaviour (Nahl,
2001). In effect, a personality structure is considered as the location for
information processing behaviour and "liking/non-liking" would be related to
(1) affect (negative: uncertainty, confusion, doubt; positive: confidence,
self-efficacy, and so on), and (2) cognitive processing preferences
(preferential intake styles, and expression or output styles). The model
presented in Figure 1 addresses the cognitive components, primarily. The
"non-liking" could be viewed as the affective component. Of interest in this
study is the relationship between the two—the affective and the cognitive.
Results
Non-liking Rates
To obtain a representation of the attitudes towards the various pedagogical
techniques the ratings were collapsed into two categories: Non-likers and
Likers. The percentages or respondents who were Non-likers within each of
the 11 techniques was taken as the non-liking measure and then graphed for
comparison purposes (see Figure 2).
As may be seen in the figure the least amount of non-liking
(approximately 20% of respondents) was expressed with respect to the "Little
Stories." Ironically, the most non-liking was expressed with respect to the
"Big Story" (approximately 62%). This contrast was a striking aspect of the
figure. Techniques that were getting non-liking responses from about 40% of
the group were the "Nano-lessons," the "WEB Resources," and the "Gimmicks."
Techniques that were getting non-liking responses from about 30% of the
group were the "WEB Outline," the "WEB Tour" and the PowerPoint Presentation
of "Research." The technology, generally, fared better than the gimmicks and
Nano-lessons, which is a plus for technology. However, the "Little Stories"
received the most support indicating that technology is not the winner in
this race. Remember though, the stories were told with technology—they were
embedded in technology and they had technology embedded within them. The mix
may make the strongest format.
On the one hand, it is surprising that no one technique appealed to more
than 80% of the group, at best. Though these techniques rely heavily on the
technology advocated to make learning more palatable to today’s "media
generation," there was still a substantial non-liking rate. Moreover, this
was the first class in the students’ program—a time when optimism would be
expected to be at its peak. Yet the non-liking rates ranged from 20% to 62%.
Non-liking Determinants
To explore these non-liking rates further the available data were
configured in terms of a model involving information-intake style,
demographics, and information-expression style (see Figure 1) and then
examined using logistic regression analyses. In these analyses the five
input variables, the four demographic variables, and the five output
variables were utilized as the model to distinguish between those who liked
a particular pedagogical technique as opposed to those who did not.
Pre-Class Activities
Music. A test of the full model (the 14 predictor variables)
against a constant-only model was not reliable,
Χ2(28) = 37.31, p > .05, indicating that the
full model did not predict pre-class music-liking. Moreover, an examination
of the Wald criterion showed that none of the variables reliably predicted
music-liking.
Pre-Class PowerPoint. A test of the full model against a
constant-only model was not reliable, Χ2(28)
= 37.91, p > .05, indicating that the full model did not predict
pre-class PowerPoint liking. An examination of the Wald criterion showed
that Gender (Wald = 10.65, p < .01; Odds Ratio = .368) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from males), and Fiction-Types (Wald = 3.88, p <
.05; Odds Ratio = .757) was a reliable predictor (poorer rating from those
liking fiction).
Sound-Bite Activities
Little-Stories. A test of the full model against a constant-only
model was reliable, Χ2(28)
= 53.94, p < .01, indicating that the full model did distinguish
between those liking Little-Stories and those who didn’t. The variance
accounted for was small (Nagelkerke’s R Square = .175). Prediction success
for classification showed an overall success rate of 82.1%. An examination
of the Wald criterion showed that Gender (Wald = 10.61, p < .01; Odds Ratio
= .374) was a reliable predictor (better rating from females).
Sample Projects. Here also a test of the full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 48.49, p
< .01, indicating that the full model did distinguish between those liking
Project Samples and those who didn’t (variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R
Square = .155). Prediction success for classification showed an overall
success rate of 80.6%. The Wald criterion showed that Gender (Wald = 6.21, p
< .01; Odds Ratio = .467) was a reliable predictor (better rating from
females), Time (Wald = 4.65, p < .05; Odds Ratio = .793) was a reliable
predictor (better rating earlier in the day), and Kinesiology Major (Wald =
5.16, p < .05; Odds Ratio = .17) was a reliable predictor (poorer rating
from Kinesiology Majors).
Nano-lessons. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 49.07, p
< .01, distinguishing between those liking Nano-lessons and those who didn’t
(variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .14). Prediction success
for classification showed an overall success rate of 67.6%. The Wald
criterion showed that Science-Types (Wald = 4.88, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
1.297) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Science-Types), Time (Wald
= 3.89, p < .05; Odds Ratio = .842) was a reliable predictor (better rating
earlier in the day), and Arts-Types (Wald = 5.74, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
1.33) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Arts-Types).
Gimmicks. The full model was not reliable,
Χ2(28) = 29.41, p
> .05, and therefore, did not distinguish between those liking Gimmicks and
those who didn’t. An examination of the Wald criterion showed that
Science-Types (Wald = 4.37, p < .01; Odds Ratio = 1.276) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from Science-Types).
WEB Activities
WEB Outline. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 43.35, p
< .05 (variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .127). Prediction
success for classification showed an overall success rate of 72%. The Wald
criterion showed that Technical-Types (Wald = 3.7, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
1.27) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Technical-Types), Time (Wald
= 4.43, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 1.22) was a reliable predictor (better rating
earlier in the day), and TV-Types (Wald = 15.23, p < .001; Odds Ratio = 1.6)
was a reliable predictor (better rating from TV-Types).
WEB Tour. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 47.79, p < .01 (variance accounted
for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .147). Prediction success for classification
showed an overall success rate of 74.9%. The Wald criterion showed that
Psychology-Types (Wald = 10.33, p < .001; Odds Ratio = 1.63) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from Psychology-Types), and News-Types (Wald =
7.93, p < .01; Odds Ratio = 1.39) was a reliable predictor (better rating
from News-Types).
WEB Resources. The full model was reliable,
Χ2(28) = 42.65, p
< .05 (variance accounted for: Nagelkerke’s R Square = .119). Prediction
success for classification revealed an overall success rate of 64%. An
examination of the Wald criterion showed that Majors was relevant where
Performing-Arts Majors (Wald = 5.84, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 4.36) was a
reliable predictor (better rating from Performing-Arts-Types), History
Majors (Wald = 7.38, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 5.71) was a reliable predictor
(better rating from History Majors), Criminology Majors (Wald = 6.15, p <
.05; Odds Ratio = 18.34) was a reliable predictor (better rating from
Criminology Majors), Science Majors (Wald = 5.06, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
3.64) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Science Majors), and
News-Types (Wald = 4.28, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 1.24) was a reliable
predictor (better rating from News-Types).
Traditional Activities
Research Review. A test of the full model against a constant-only
model was not reliable, Χ2(28)
= 34.13, p > .05. The Wald criterion showed that History Majors (Wald
= 4.25, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 4.42) was a reliable predictor (better rating
from History Majors), and Science Majors (Wald = 5.32, p < .05; Odds Ratio =
4.32) was a reliable predictor (better rating from Science Majors).
The "Big" Story. The full model was not reliable,
Χ2(28) = 29.44, p
> .05. The Wald criterion showed that Literate-Types (Wald = 4.45, p < .05;
Odds Ratio = 1.25) was a reliable predictor (better rating from
Literate-Types), and Fiction-Types (Wald = 4.55, p < .05; Odds Ratio = 1.27)
was a reliable predictor (better rating from Fiction-Types).
Discussion
The most striking aspect of these data is a clear and substantial
non-liking rate for a broad range of pedagogical techniques. Even the
stories—under the category of Sound-Bites—were viewed in less than positive
ways by some (about 20%). This occurred regardless of the optimal conditions
associated with the "first class" of the semester, a time when optimism
should be high. On the positive side, the least amount of non-liking was
expressed with respect to the "Little Stories," and therefore, "story,"
rather than "technology," would seem to be the more appealing technique for
students. But ironically, and more striking, was the non-liking expressed
with respect to a classic story teller like W.O. Mitchell. The non-liking
rate for this story was 62%. It seems stories may capture both the high and
the low of the non-liking responses. One difference between the two stories
was the time allotment (a few minutes versus 25 minutes) which may indicate
that a short auditory attention span for today’s adult students is a
possible source of the difference.
The WEB components also revealed substantial non-liking (28% to 42%).
Technological and media-friendly techniques may be appealing to the
majority, but it would seem that a large portion of the student population
would be somewhat averse to this format. To one who likes media this is both
surprising and disappointing. Apparently, one cannot rely, or should not
rely, on the technological media even if embedded within stories, or used as
vehicles for stories.
Fluctuations were also evident within the WEB techniques: the WEB-Outline
and the WEB-Tour were viewed more favourably than the WEB-Resources. This
difference is likely related to the personal and immediate implications of
the WEB-Outline and the WEB-Tour. These were directly relevant for course
requirements, whereas, the WEB-Resources were not "immediately" relevant,
and thus the lower rating for WEB-Resources. Generally, the pedagogical hope
offered by media needs to be tempered by the normal non-liking rates in any
student population, as well as such variables as time, relevance, and, as
may be seen below, the demographic and psychodynamic qualities of the
learner.
The model configured to try and get a better understanding of some of the
determinants of these attitudes was informative. This Input-Output
Psychodynamic Model of Learner-Processing revealed numerous potential
determinants of non-liking attitudes. The model was viewed as preliminary,
exploratory, and a broad ranging configuration of learner-processing, yet it
did allow for two levels of analysis and commentary here.
On the one hand, the model was reliable for distinguishing Likers from
Non-Likers for Sound-Bite Techniques (Little Stories, Project Samples, and
Nano-lessons) and WEB Techniques (Outline, Tour, Resources). It was not
reliable with respect to the Pre-class Activities or the Traditional
Activities. When reliable, however, the amount of variance explained was not
large (11.9% to 17.5%), so a tempered response, at best, is warranted.
On the other hand, the specific variables that were reliable predictors
are of interest in understanding why there would be differential responses
to the pedagogical techniques. This is theoretically interesting, and may
lead to more refined models of learning and teaching. Thus, there is a
rationale for considering the responses in terms of Demographics, Input
variables and Output variables. That interesting differences exist is not to
argue that instructors should try to match pedagogical technique with the
learning style of the students; this could be a logistical nightmare.
Rather, on the practical side, understanding learner differences could
inform instructors about the need for varied methods, could suggest ‘better’
methods, and could create an awareness of the seemingly problematic methods.
Learner Differences: Demographics
In terms of demographics, Gender was a reliable predictor for: (1) the
Pre-Class PowerPoint (males were more likely to like it), (2) the Little
Stories (females liked those), and (3) the Sample Projects (females liked
those). So males may gravitate to the visual and the technological, while
females are more oriented to story (the story of the "Sample Projects," and
the "Little Stories"). This seems to fit our stereotypes.
Time-of-Day was a reliable predictor with more favourable ratings earlier
in the day on (1) Nano-lessons, (2) Sample Projects, and (3) the WEB
Outline. There seems to be an attitudinal shift later in the day. What these
three techniques may have in common is their function of providing specific
information on behavioural demands. If students are more critical of these
later in the day it may be because they are using more cognitively
sophisticated processing later in the day. There is a body of literature
suggesting enhanced learning later in the day for certain types of tasks (Baddeley,
Hatter, Scott & Snashall, Blake, 1967; Folkard, 1979: Folkard & Monk, 1978;
Morton, 1986; Morton & Kershner, 1985, 1991; Tilley & Warren, 1983). A more
critical attitude, which may be linked to non-liking, is consistent with
this time-of-day literature.
Major was a reliable predictor for Sample Projects (with a poorer rating
from Kinesiology Majors). Major was also a reliable predictor for Research
(with better ratings—less non-liking—from History Majors and Science
Majors). The higher "Research" ratings for History and Science Majors make
sense. These majors would be heavily invested in research activity and thus
see value in research. The poorer rating from Kinesiology Majors may be
logically linked to the action style of this group. Preferring activity they
may find some projects too sedentary, or too formal. With respect to the
WEB-Resources, the following Majors were reliable predictors: Performing
Arts, History, Criminology, and Science (each showing more favourable
attitudes). This reveals an interesting and diverse range of background
variables that seem to impact the valuing of WEB resources. While History,
Criminology and Science majors would logically be drawn to such resources,
it is not clear what is drawing the Performing Arts majors.
Learner Characteristics: Input Variables
Fiction-Types was a reliable predictor of the non-liking towards the
Pre-Class PowerPoint. They didn’t care for it. But they did like the "Big
Story," as did those scoring higher on the Literate-Types scale. This makes
sense. Those who like literature especially should be drawn to the sense of
story—particularly with a classic storyteller like W.O. Mitchell.
TV-Types predicted WEB-Outline non-liking rate, while News-Types
predicted WEB-Tour non-liking rate. That students who like the TV (TV-Types)
and news sources (News-Types) would also appreciate the WEB is reasonable. A
computer hooked up to the WEB is somewhat homologous with the TV and News
sources. Indeed, such a tour could be construed as a form of news media, and
similar to watching TV.
Learner Characteristics: Output Variables
Here a number of reliable predictors of non-liking were evident. The
Science-Types scale predicted the Nano-lessons non-liking rate, and the
Gimmicks non-liking rate. Science-Types liked these sound-bites (the Nano-lessons
and the Gimmicks). While these do not map onto the output characteristics
mentioned in Table 7 earlier, they do have an associated quality—clear,
terse action. This might appeal to "Science-Types."
The Arts-Types scale also predicted the Nano-lessons non-liking rate. One
could speculate that the Arts-Types liked the Nano-lessons because they are
short and dramatic, and at times have performance elements attached to them.
The Technical-Types scale predicted the WEB-Outline non-liking rate. They
liked it. Technical-Types may have a preference for design, structure,
technique, and technology.
The Psychology-Types scale predicted the WEB-Tour non-liking rate.
Perhaps the Psychology-Types liked the WEB Tour because it provided
information they could use in their own outreach. In addition, it was an
educational psychology class so the information toured would align with
their interests.
Two things are evident here: (1) an apparently complex interplay between
the pedagogical technique one experiences, one’s preferred modes of
information intake, one’s background, and one’s perceived or preferential
output style, and (2) a non-liking with respect to various pedagogical
techniques that is influenced by this complex interplay. Even highly rated,
and highly valued, techniques have more than their fair share of detractors,
and the detractors are interesting in unique ways. Thus an answer to the
question "Why do some resist technology in the classroom?" would emerge as:
(1) there is a fluctuating, but striking, resident "non-liking" rate for any
particular technique in a large group, and (2) there are logical
psychodynamic and experiential determinants that influence
"liking/non-liking."
The Positive Twist
Such information on non-liking may be of value to teachers. Some may wish
to utilize techniques associated with lower rates of non-liking. Others may
wish to try and match learners and techniques. However, these are not the
recommendations of interest here; yet there is an interesting positive twist
that emerges from these findings.
Such information may be of value to administrators. Knowing that there is
a residual non-liking rate in a body of university students may temper
judgments made of faculty members (regarding renewal, promotion and tenure)
which are based on student ratings. But this is not the positive twist
either.
The positive twist is the argument that such non-liking is valuable. It
is encouraging, and it should be encouraged, cultivated, nurtured and
extended. Teachers, perhaps, should be designing their courses to generate
more non-liking, not less. The rationale for this argument emerges from (1)
empirical studies (e.g., Milgram, 1974), (2) informants (e.g., Garfinkle,
2000; Turkle, 1995), and (3) the critics of technology/technique (e.g.,
Ellul, 1981; Postman, 1992). With respect to empirical studies, consider
Milgram’s famous experiments where subjects were requested to administer
electric shock to a person (an accomplice of the experimenter) in response
to directives from the authority figure (the researcher). Even when the
person receiving the shock appeared to be under great duress many of the
subjects continued to administer shock in response to the authority figure’s
request. About 35% refused—echoing an interesting rate of non-liking—a
similar rate as the non-liking with respect to pedagogy.
With respect to informants, the implication of the surveillance we are
exposed to as a function of technology alarms us. We are at risk, and the
critics are a key defence. When Turkle (1995) informs us of what is actually
happening on the WEB, we become more suspicious and welcome those who adopt
the more cynical view. When Garfinkle alerts us to the war on privacy by
government, business, and neighbours, we call for more critical awareness.
We see that there are critics operating in the community and wonder how we
can encourage the more critical and the more cynical view in our students.
One critic, Ellul, argues that educators need to be teaching students to
live "in" technology/technique, but "against" technology/technique. He sees
"real" danger. Whereas some (see Postman, 1992) at least note that the
computer is the metaphor of our age—the paradigmatic metaphor—Ellul has a
broader vision; for him technology/technique is more than metaphor. The
place occupied by "capital" for the past 200 years has now been supplanted
by "technology/technique," as Ellul sees it. Power, therefore, has a new
substrate; it has shifted to "technology/technique." In addition to this
profound social and political effect, there are epistemological effects; he
contends that technology/technique has two major epistemological effects—it
suppresses the subject, and it suppresses meaning. "The means has entirely
replaced the meaning" (Ellul, 1980, p.254). Is this what McLuhan meant when
contending that the "medium is the message?" The means becomes the end. If
he is right, and he makes a good case for this, it is the critics—the
cynics—who will help restore and strengthen the subject, and recover meaning
in the presence of an ever widening exercise of "technology/technique." If
he is right such resident non-liking, or critical attitude, or cynicism,
should be cultivated—perhaps developing the self-appointed critic, the
cross-examiner, "the devil’s advocate," the Socratic gadfly, the wearer of
deBono’s "Black Hat," the contrarian, and so on.
References
Armstrong, A. & Casement, C. (1998). The child and the machine. Toronto:
Key Porter Books.
Baddeley, A. D., Hatter, J. E., Scott, D., & Snashall, A. (1970). Memory
and time of day. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 22,
605-609.
Blake, M. J. F. (1967). Time of day effects on performance in a range of
tasks. Psychonomic Science, 9, 349-350.
Ellul, J. (1964). The technological society. New York: Vintage
Books.
Ellul, J. (1980). The technological system. New York: Continuum.
Ellul, J. (1981). Perspectives on our age, Jacques Ellul speaks on his
life and work. Editor W. H. Vanderburg. Toronto: House of Anansi Press
Ltd.
Flumen, A. F. & Flumen, L. B. (1979). WISCOS and WPPSICLES. Journal of
School Psychology, 17, 82-85.
Folkard, S. (1979). Time of day and level of processing. Memory and
Cognition, 7, 247-252.
Folkard, S. & Monk, H. (1978). Time of day effects in immediate and
delayed memory. In M. M. Gruenberg, P. E. Morris, & R. N. Sykes (Eds.),
Practical aspects of memory. London: Academic Press.
Milgram, S. (1974). Obedience to authority: An experimental view.
New York: Harper & Row.
Morton, L. L. (1986). A single-subject study of the effects of time on
task and time of day on productivity and achievement in a dysgraphic student
,. Canadian Journal for Exceptional Children, 3, 23-28.
Morton, L. L. & Kershner, J. R. (1985). Time-of-day effects upon
children's memory and analogical reasoning. The Alberta Journal of
Educational Research, 31, 26-34.
Morton, L. L. & Kershner, J. R. (1991). Time-of-day effects on
neuropsychological behaviors as measured by dichotic listening . The
International Journal of Neuroscience, 59, 241-251.
Nahl. D. (2001). A conceptual framework for explaining information
behavior. SIMILE 1. Retrieved November 30th, 2001 from
http://www.utpjournals.com/jour.ihtml?lp=simile/issue2/issue2toc.html
Negroponte, N. (1995). Being digital. New York: Vintage
Books.
Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms. New York: Basic Books.
Papert, S. (1993). The children’s machine. New York: Basic Books.
Postman, N. (1992). Technopoly. New York: Vintage Books.
Stoll. C. (1999). High tech heretic. New York: Doubleday.
Tilley, A. & Warren, P. (1983). Retrieval from semantic memory at
different times of day. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
Memory, and Cognition, 9, 718-724.
Turkle, S. (1995). Life on the screen. New York: Touchstone.
|
|
|
|
|
|