Cognitive and Metacognitive Aspects of Mathematical
Problem Solving: An Instructional Intervention
Introduction
A primary goal of mathematics teaching and learning is to develop the
ability to solve problems (Wilson, Fernandez & Hadaway, 1993; Lester, 1980;
National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics, 1978). We solve problems
every time we achieve something without knowing beforehand how to do so.
Problem solving involves the process of confronting a novel situation
formulating connections between given facts, identifying the goal, and
exploring possible strategies for researching the goal (Szetula, 1992).
Mathematics teachers have voiced concern about the inability of students to
solve any problems but the most routine problems even though they have
mastered all the required computational skills (Lester, Garofalo & Kroll,
1989). Until recently researchers have attributed problem solving
difficulties almost exclusively to cognitive aspects. One aspect getting
considerably more attention of late is metacognition. Metacognition refers
to knowledge and control one has of one’s cognitive functioning. With
respect to mathematical performance this includes knowing one’s strengths
and weaknesses together with an awareness of one’s repertoire of strategies
and how these can enhance performance (Lester et al., 1989). Lester et al
(1989) contend that for students struggling to become competent problem
solvers, the complexity of problem solving is compounded by the fact that
most of them do not receive adequate instruction, either in quality or
quantity. Many mathematics teachers have received little or no systematic
training in problem solving. They suggest two reasons why problem solving
competence is so difficult for students to develop: i) problem solving is a
complex cognitive activity which requires much more than just direct
application of some mathematical content knowledge. Further, these cognitive
activities are influenced by a number of non-cognitive factors. ii) students
need to be given appropriate opportunities to become proficient problem
solvers. They need to be given carefully designed problem solving
instruction and they need to be given extensive problem solving experience.
The purpose of this proposed study is to develop an instructional
intervention to promote the development of cognitive and metacognitive
problem solving skills.
Cognitive Aspects of Problem Solving
Research has been carried out both by cognitive psychologists and
mathematics educators in an attempt to understand the cognitive aspects of
mathematical problem solving. George Polya (1957) an eminent mathematician
has written extensively on the topic of mathematical problem solving. He
developed a model of problem solving, which includes the following
processes: understanding the problem, devising a plan, carrying out the plan
and looking back. Much research on problem solving has been based on his
work (Schoenfeld, 1987).
A current approach to studying cognition is the information processing
model. This model has three major components: problem task environment,
working memory and long term memory (Silver, 1987, Stiff et al., 1993). The
task environment is the structure of facts, concepts and interrelationships
that make up the problem. Working memory or short term memory is where most
of the cognitive action is occurring. Working memory maintains an internal
representation of the current state of cognitive activity. It is also where
information obtained from the external problem task environment interacts
with knowledge retrieved from long term memory. Mathematical knowledge, such
as basic facts, processes, generalized problem types, schemas, algorithms as
well as metacognitive knowledge is stored in long term memory. Information
processing involves controlling information into and out of working memory,
recognizing, comparing and manipulating symbols in working memory, and
storing information in long term memory (Silver, 1987). Information can be
stored in long term memory only after it is has been processed in working
memory and it can be used in thinking only after being retrieved from long
term memory and placed in working memory.
Most researchers taking a cognitive science approach to learning agree
that individuals construct their own knowledge bases (Silver and Marshall,
1990). The constructivist approach basically states that students construct
knowledge to fit what they already know or believe about the world. In order
to solve a problem one must construct an understanding of the problem that
connects his or her present knowledge with the task requirements of the
problem. These problem representations are viewed as central to the problem
solving process (Silver and Marshall, 1990). Problem representation is
linked to Polya’s first stage of problem solving, understanding the problem.
Failure to solve problems can often be traced back to failing to understand
the problem and consequently failing to construct adequate problem
representations (Silver & Marshall, 1990; Mayer & Hegarty, 1996). "The major
creative work in solving word problems rests in understanding what the
problem means." (Mayer & Hegarty, 1996).
Metacognitive Aspects of Mathematical
Problem Solving
Metacognition has been described as ‘reflections on cognition’ or
‘thinking about one’s own thinking’ (Schoenfeld, 1987). It includes
awareness of how one learns; awareness of what one does and does not
understand; knowledge of how to use available information to achieve a goal;
ability to judge the cognitive demands of a particular task; knowledge of
what strategies to use for what purposes; and assessment of one’s progress
both during and after performance (Flavell, 1979). It is these skills that
enable one to plan, monitor and evaluate performance throughout the
execution of a task. Simply stated, cognition can be described as those
skills involved in doing, whereas metacognition involves those skills
required to choose and plan what to do, as well as, to monitor what is being
done (Garofalo & Lester, 1985).
While teaching basic skills may seem to be the most straightforward way
to improve problem solving performance, research indicates that knowledge of
basic skills alone is not sufficient (Mayer, 1998). Problem solvers in
addition to requiring specific math skills, also require the ability to
mange these skills. Schoenfeld (1985) in videotaping students solving
problems aloud in pairs, observed that even when the students demonstrated
mastery of the course material, they still failed to solve familiar problems
correctly. He would observe students solving a geometry problem where they
immediately chose a solution method without discussing if or why the method
was appropriate. When they encountered difficulties they did not stop to
consider whether they were on the right track. Further, when students got
stuck, they often moved on to another strategy without reflecting on what
had gone wrong. As a result they would abandon good ideas along with the bad
ones. Schoenfeld (1985) describes good ’control’ and not necessarily always
making the right decisions, but being able to realize that a strategy in not
working, and to consider alternatives. In the above case, the students had
the resources to solve the problems, but they were unable to apply them
successfully because they lacked the knowledge of how to regulate their
thinking.
Schoenfeld (1987) investigated the metacognitive skills of novice and
experts in problem solving. His study found that novice students quickly
choose a solution strategy and then spent their remaining time in the
execution of the strategy. In contrast, Schoenfeld found that the expert
problem solvers spent most of their time analyzing the problem making sure
that they understood it. The approach of the experts was to constantly ask
themselves if the chosen strategy was working and if the answer was ‘no’ to
search for a new strategy.
Swanson (1990) investigated whether high levels of metacognitive
knowledge about problem solving could compensate for low overall aptitude.
He looked at students in grades 4 and 5 and based on their scores on the
Cognitive Abilities Test (CAT) categorized them as high-aptitude (scores
above 120) and low-aptitude (scores below 105) students. The children were
also administered a metacognitive questionnaire to assess their level of
metacognition in the general area of problem solving. Children were
categorized as high and low metacognitive performers. The problem solving
tasks the children were given were specific to mathematics specific but were
general tasks frequently used in Piagetian studies. One task involved a
pendulum where students were given string of 3 lengths and 3 sets of weights
and they were asked to identify the factor that determined the frequency of
oscillations. The second, combinatorial task, required the children to
discover a combination of 3 clear, colourless liquids that yielded a
particular colour when an indicator was added. Swanson found that the high-metacognitive
individuals outperformed the lower metacognitive individuals in problem
solving regardless of their overall aptitude level. Based on these findings
the authors suggest that high metacognitive skills can compensate, to some
degree, for overall ability.
Cognitive and Metacognitive Framework
Garofalo and Lester (1985) presented a cognitive and metacognitive
framework for studying mathematics performance. The framework (see Appendix
A) comprises four categories of activities involved in performing a
mathematical task such as problem solving: orientation, organization,
execution and verification. The framework does not list all possible
cognitive and metacognitive behaviors that might occur , rather it specifies
key points where metacognitive decisions are likely to influence cognitive
actions. The orientation category includes strategic behaviors to assess and
understand the problem, as well as, strategies to obtain an appropriate
representation of the problem situation. The organization category includes
the planning of one’s behavior and choice of actions. The execution phase
involves the problem solver regulating behavior to conform to the plans and
monitoring the implementation of the strategy. The verification category
includes evaluating the decisions made as well as the outcomes of the
executed plan. Included in this category would be evaluating the adequacy
and reasonableness of the solution. These phases are related to Polya’s four
phases of problem solving. The cognitive-metacognitive framework is intended
to serve as a tool for analyzing metacognitive aspects of mathematical
performance (Garofalo & Lester,1985). In addition, they believe that the
mathematics education community needs to adapt more of a metacognitive
perspective in regard to the development of instructional treatments. If the
critical role of metacognition is made clearer, educators will be in a
better position to incorporate metacognitive aspects into mathematics
education.
Instructional Implications
Teaching mathematics is a complex task. How a student learns or acquires
new skills and information needs to impact on how teachers should teach
(Stiff et al., 1993). Based on the constructivist approach to learning,
teachers cannot expect transfer of mathematical concepts and relationships
to occur by simply telling their students what they know. At one time the
best advice from the research literature to teachers given the task of
developing student’s problem solving abilities was that problem solving
should be taught by giving students lots of problems to solve (Silver &
Marshall, 1990). Recent research suggests a more refined approach. While
extensive practice in solving appropriate problems is a necessary
requirement for the development of expertise, it is not sufficient.
Gourgey (1998) asks the question how are students able to achieve good
grades in mathematics courses and then demonstrate such poor ability to
solve the same problems? It is common practice, she notes, for similar
problem types to be tested together within a particular lesson. Students, as
a result, can apply these procedures mechanically without having to
understand them. In addition, it could also be that instruction which
focuses on performing techniques could be neglecting the ‘when’ and ‘why’ of
strategy use. If this is the case, students may not be internalizing the
skills that would enable them to analyze a problem and to draw upon their
knowledge to solve it (Gourgey, 1998).
The learning of problem solving has been shown to be facilitated by
student discussion. Hart (1993) undertook a study involving seventh grade
students who were placed into cooperative groups. The students were asked to
solve a four part applied problem. The students had been exposed to the
mathematical concepts necessary for solving the problem, but had received no
formal instruction on problem solving strategies. One of the factors shown
to enhance problem solving performance was group collaboration. The
collective experience of the group frequently supplied background
information that individual students did not possess. In addition, a second
benefit of group work was that the challenge and disbelief of peers acted as
a form of external monitoring when self monitoring was not apparent.
Although students seldom questioned their own strategies, they occasionally
did challenge each other. Such encounters seemed to force them to examine
their own knowledge, strategies and beliefs more closely (Hart, 1993). She
found that one of the factors that impeded problem solving performance was
lack of individual monitoring or regulation of cognitive activity.
Mayer (1998) suggests that a metacognitive approach to the learning of
problem solving skills should involve the modeling of how and when to use
strategies in realistic academic tasks. If problem solving performance is to
improve, students need to learn how to represent problems within the context
of actually trying to solve it. Mayer believes that the most successful
instructional technique for teaching students how to control their
mathematical problem solving strategies is having a competent problem solver
describe his/her thinking processes as he/she solves a real problem in an
academic setting.
Incorporating the above instructional strategies, student discussion and
modeling among others, Lester et al.(1989) undertook a study to look at the
role of metacognition in seventh grader’s problem solving and explore the
extent to which these students can be taught to be more strategic and
self-aware of their problem solving behaviors. Prior to instructional
intervention all students in two grade 7 classes were administered a
pre-test and a parallel version of this test was given to all students
within a week after the end of the instructional phase. The primary
assessment was conducted analyzing videotapes of individual students and
pairs of students working on multi-step problems. The students were also
interviewed on their mathematical knowledge, strategies, decisions, beliefs
and affects. The instruction was presented by one of the investigators 3
days per week for a period of 12 weeks. The instruction consisted of 3
concurrent components: the teacher as external monitor, the teacher as
facilitator of students’ metacognitive development, and the teacher as a
model of a metacognitively aware problem solver.
The teacher as external monitor, emphasized the 10 teaching actions for
the teacher to engage in (see Appendix B). The teacher directs whole class
discussions about a problem that is to be solved; observes, questions and
guides students as they work either individually or in small groups to solve
the problem and leads a whole-class discussion about students’ solution
efforts. The teacher as facilitor of students’ metacognitive development,
assumes this role when he/she asks, questions and devises assignments that
require students to analyze their mathematical performance, points out
aspects of mathematics and mathematical activity that have bearing on
performance, and helps students build a repertoire of heuristics and control
strategies along with knowledge of their usefulness.
The teacher as a model, involves teacher explicitly demonstrating
regulatory decisions and actions while solving problems for students in the
classroom. The intent is to give students the opportunity to observe the
monitoring strategies used by an ‘expert’.
Lester et al.(1989) found that the more successful problem solvers were
better able to monitor and regulate their problem solving activity than were
the poorer problem solvers, with the difference being most apparent during
the orientation phase. They also found that effective monitoring requires
students knowing not only what and when to monitor, but also how to do so.
The authors believe that students can be taught what and when to monitor
relatively easily but helping them acquire the skills needed to monitor
effectively is more difficult. Metacognitive training is likely to be most
effective when it takes place in the context of learning specific
mathematical concepts and skills.
Adibnia and Putt (1998) designed a study to investigate the effects of an
instructional intervention based on Garofalo and Lester ‘s(1985) cognitive-metacognitive
framework. Specifically, they investigated the effect of the teaching
intervention on the mathematical problem solving performance of grade 6
students of different ability levels. The instructional intervention
consisted of fourteen 90 minute lessons involving a variety of process
problems. Lessons plans were designed using the Garofalo and Lester
framework. The focus was on developing students’ metacognitive behavior
primarily, to think about their own thinking and to become aware of the
regulation and monitoring of their activities. The instruction had three
components: the teacher as external monitor, the teacher as facilitator of
students’ metacognitive development, and the teacher as a model of a
metacognitively aware problem solver similar to that of Lester et al (1989).
In addition, the daily lesson plans consisted of four phases – Orientation,
Organization, Execution and Verification – which were followed by the
teacher.
To measure the effectiveness of the instructional intervention, the
students were administered a process problem test before and after the
instruction. The tests were scored using an analytic scoring scheme (see
Appendix C). The scoring assigned a maximum of two points to each of the
cognitive and metacognitive phases, namely Orientation, Organization,
Execution and Verification.
The authors found that the instructional intervention was more effective
for above-average than for below-average students. Above-average students
were found to make greater use of the cognitive-metacogntive strategies
following the instructional intervention. The study did not address,
however, how the use of these strategies impacted their final grade in the
course.
Rationale for the Current Study
In 1999, The Ontario Curriculum, Grades 9 and 10: Mathematics, was
introduced. The new curriculum embeds the learning of mathematics in the
solving of problems based on real-life situations. It assumes that students
will be called upon to explain their reasoning in writing, or orally to the
teacher, to the class, or to a few other students. An important part of
every course in the mathematics program is the process of inquiry, where
students develop a systematic method for exploring new problems or
unfamiliar situations. The inquiry process is a major strategy that
underlies teaching and learning of mathematics at all levels and all grades.
The new curriculum is based on two essential elements: expectations and
achievement levels. Expectations are the knowledge and skills students are
expected to demonstrate. These expectations are subject, grade and level
(academic/applied) specific. The achievement levels describe four possible
levels of student achievement. In the area of mathematics these levels focus
on four knowledge/skill categories: knowledge/understanding,
thinking/inquiry/problem solving, communication and application. Levels 1
and 2 identify achievement that falls below the expectations specified for
the grade but are passable levels of achievement. For example, a student is
not passing the course would be below Level 1. Level 3 identifies
achievement that meets the expectations and is considered the provincial
standard. Level 4 achievement is that which surpasses the expectations
(Ministry of Education, 1999).
To measure the extent to which the curriculum expectations are being met
across the province of Ontario, standardized testing was introduced. This
testing is administered by The Education Quality and Accountability Office (E.Q.A.O.)
whose mandate is to provide reliable data about student achievement in
Ontario’s publicly funded schools. In the most recent Ontario Provincial
Report on Achievement, 200-2001: English-Language Secondary Schools, are the
results of the Grade 9 Assessment of Mathematics, administered for the first
time in the province. The assessment is based on the expectations for the
entire Grade 9 curriculum for applied and academic courses.
On this first test, in terms of overall achievement 89% of the students
in the province taking mathematics at the academic level achieved at a
‘passable level of achievement’ (Level 1 to 4) and 49% of these students
achieved at or above the provincial standard. The results for the students
taking mathematics at the applied level are an area of concern. Sixty two
percent of the students at the applied level achieved at a ‘passable level
of achievement’ and only 13% of all the students at the applied level
achieved at or above the provincial standard. Specifically with respect to
the category of Thinking/Inquiry/Problem Solving 80% students studying at
the academic level achieved a passable level of achievement and 40% achieved
at or above the provincial standard. At the applied level of study, 52%
achieved at a passable level of achievement and 10% of the students achieved
at or above the provincial standard in the area of problem solving. Problem
solving at the applied level is a particular area of concern in the
province.
Aims of the Study and Proposed Research
Questions:
The aims of the proposed study are as follows:
1. To design and implement an instructional intervention based on
Garofalo and Lester’s (1985) cognitive and metacognitive framework.
2. To investigate the effect of this instructional intervention on
mathematical problem solving as measured by students’ scores on the
E.Q.A.O. Grade 9 Mathematics Assessment.
Potential research questions include:
Do students who receive instruction on cognitive-metacognitive
behaviors achieve at a higher level on the problem solving component of
the Grade 9 E.Q.A.O Mathematics Assessment than students who do not
receive instruction on cognitive-metacognitive behaviors?
a. What metacognitive behaviors do Grade 9 students exhibit when
solving mathematical problems before cognitive-metacognitive
instruction?
b. What metacognitive behaviors do Grade 9 students exhibit when
solving mathematical problems after cognitive-metacognitive instruction?
c. Are there significant differences in the metacognitive behaviors
exhibited by students before and after receiving cognitive-metacognitive
instruction?
Are there differences in the metacognitive behaviors of students
studying mathematics at the academic vs. applied level of study?
a. Are there differences in the metacognitive behaviors of successful
vs. non-successful students at academic level of study?
b. Are there differences in the metacognitive behaviors of successful
vs. non-successful students at the applied level of study?
Methodology
This study will involve Grade 9 students at 2 urban high schools of
similar demographics and enrollment. All students taking Grade 9 Mathematics
at the academic and applied level will be involved (provided parental
permission is obtained). There is expected to be approximately 20 classes of
mathematics, 12 Academic and 8 Applied level classes.
Semester I
Semester II
Academic- 6 classes (control)
Academic – 6 classes (experimental)
Applied – 4 classes (control)
Applied – 4 classes (experimental)
All classes will be taught by their regular teacher.
Pre and Post Tests:
All classes will be administered a pre-test during the first week of the
semester and a post-test will be administered the final week of the
semester. While the questions on the two tests will not be identical they
will have a similar format. Each test will consist of 8 problems, 2 from
each of the 4 strands in the Grade 9 curriculum. The questions will be
modeled after those used on the E.Q.A.O. Grade 9 Mathematics Assessment. The
Board’s Mathematics Consultant will develop a scoring scheme based on the
Grade 9 Achievement Chart. In addition, all the problems will be assessed
using the 8 point system for scoring responses (see Appendix C) used by
Adibnia & Putt (1982) which they adapted from Charles & Lester (1985).
Mathematics Information Processing Scale (M.I.P.S.)
Bressant (1997) developed a instrument called Mathematics Information
Processing Scale (MIPS). The scale explores: learning strategies for
statistics or mathematics related content, metacognitive problem solving
skills and attentional deployment in evaluative contexts. The 87 scale
elements were derived from research into: student learning approaches;
metacognitive problem solving; cognitive-attentional models of test anxiety.
Students used a likert-type scale to indicate how well statements describe
their study motives, strategies, beliefs and experiences. Bressant used this
scale with first year University students studying statistics. With
permission from the author, the scale will be adapted and used with the
grade 9 mathematics students. The adapted scale will be piloted this school
year. While this research is primarily interested in investigating cognitive
and metacognitive components of mathematical problem solving, all components
of this instrument will be administered. The additional data obtained may be
used for future studies.
Instructional Intervention
A teacher in-service on the instructional intervention, based Garofolo &
Lester’s (1985) cognitive metacognitive framework, will be planned and
designed by the researcher. The in-service will be delivered to the Grade 9
Semester II mathematics teachers towards the end of the first semester and
will be delivered by the mathematics consultant.
The intervention will emphasize the previously mentioned 3 roles of the
classroom teacher that of external monitor, facilitator and model. A brief
explanation of each follows.
Teacher as External Monitor: consists of the 10 teaching actions for the
teacher to engage in (see Appendix B). The teacher directs whole class
discussions about a problem that is to be solved; observes, questions and
guides students as they work either individually or in small groups to solve
the problem and leads a whole class discussion about student’s solution
efforts.
The Teacher as Facilitator of Student’s Metacognitive Development: when
the teacher assumes this role, he/she asks questions and devises assignments
that require students to analyze their mathematical performance, points out
aspects of mathematics and mathematical ability that have bearing on
performance; and helps students build a repertoire of heuristics and control
strategies along with knowledge of their usefulness. Aooperative group
setting will be used.
The teacher as a Model: involves the teacher explicitly demonstrating
regulatory decisions and actions while solving problems for students in the
classroom. The intent is to give students the opportunity to observe the
monitoring strategies used by an ‘expert’. In addition, the teacher directs
a discussion with the class about their observations of the expert’s
behavior.
A model lesson plan consisting of the four phases of Garofalo & Lester’s
(1985) framework – orientation, organization, execution and verification –
will be developed similar to that of Adibnia & Putt (1998). During the
orientation phase, the teacher will be helping students understand the
aspects of the problem. In the organization phase the teacher assists
students in finding strategies that lead to a possible solution. During the
execution phase the students will be encouraged to work in small groups to
implement their plan for solving the problem. The teacher will be monitoring
what students are doing and giving them hints in the form of questions to
help groups that are having difficulty. The teacher also reminds the
students to check the accuracy of their calculations and the reasonableness
of their final answer. In the verification phase students will individually
check their work and share their solutions with others. Students will grade
each group’s solution using the 8-point system for scoring responses (see
Appendix C) during this phase.
Verifying the Implementation: The Mathematics consultant will visit the
classrooms through the semester using a checklist to verify that the
implementation is taking place.
E.Q.A.O. Grade 9 Mathematics Assessment: all students will be
participating in the province wide test the second last week of the
semester. Permission will be sought to obtain the test results. In addition,
the students’ final mark in the class will be obtained. All participants
will be informed that their scores will be kept confidential and anonymity
will be assured.
In addition to the previously mentioned research questions, also of
interest is the correlation between the EQAO scores, final marks in the
class and the post-test scores.
References:
Adibnia, A. & Putt, I.J. (1998). Teaching problem solving to year 6
students: a new approach. Mathematics Education Research Journal, 10(3),
42 – 58.
Bressant, K.C. (1997). The development and validation of scores on the
mathematics information processing scale (MIPS). Educational and
Psychological Measurement, 57(5), 841 – 857.
Education Quality and Accountability Office (2001). Ontario Provincial
Report on Achievement: 2000 – 2001 English-Language Secondary Schools.
Toronto, ON: Queen’s Printer for Ontario.
Flavell, J.H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area
of cognitive developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34(10),
906 – 911.
Garofalo, J. & Lester, F.K. (1985). Metacognition, cognitive monitoring
and mathematical performance. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 16(3), 163 – 176.
Gourgey, A.F. (1998). Metacognition in basic skills instruction.
Instructional Science, 26, 81 – 96.
Hart, L.C. (1993). Some factors that impede or enhance performance in
mathematical problem solving. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 24, 167 – 171.
Leahey, T. & Harris, R.J. (1997). Learning and Cognition. Upper
Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
Lester, F.K. (1980). Problem solving: Is it a problem? In M.M. Lindquist
(Ed), Selected issues in mathematics education (pp. 29 – 45).
Berkeley, CA: McCutchan Publishing Corporation.
Lester, F.K. (1983). Trends and issues in mathematical problem-solving
research. In R. Lesh & M. Landou (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematics
concepts and processes (pp. 229 – 261). New York: Academic Press.
Lester, F.K., Garofalo, J., & Kroll, D.L. (1989). The role of
metacognition in mathematical problem solving: Final Report. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service No. ED 314 255)
Martinez, M.E. (1998). What is problem solving? Phi Delta Kappan,
605 – 609.
Mayer, R.E. (1998). Cognitive, metacognitive, and motivational aspects of
problem solving. Instructional Science, 26, 49 – 63.
Mayer, R.E. & Hegarty, M. (1996). The process of understanding
mathematical problems. In R.J. Sternberg, T. Ben-Zeev (Eds). The nature
of mathematical thinking. (pp. 29 – 53). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Ministry of Education and Training (1999). The Ontario Curriculum
Grades 9 and 10: Mathematics. Toronto, ON: Queen’s Printer for Ontario.
National Council of Supervisors of Mathematics (1978). Position paper on
basic mathematical skills. Mathematics Teacher, 71(2), 147 – 52.
Polya, G. (1957). How to solve it (2nd edition). New York:
Doubleday.
Randhawa, B.S. (1994). Theory, research and assessment of mathematical
problem solving. The Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 40(2),
213 – 231.
Schoenfeld, A.H. (1987). What’s all the fuss about metacognition? In A.H.
Schoenfeld. Cognitive science and mathematics education (pp. 189 –
215). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Schoenfeld, A.H. (1985). Mathematical problem solving. San
Diego,CA: Academic Press Incorporated.
Silver, E.A. (1987). Foundations of cognitive theory and research for
mathematics problem solving. In A.H. Schoenfeld. Cognitive science and
mathematics education (pp. 33 – 60). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Silver, E.A. (1979). Student perceptions of relatedness among
mathematical verbal problems. Journal for research in mathematics
education. 10, 195 – 210.
Silver, E.A. and Marshall, S.P. (1990). Mathematical and scientific
problem solving: Findings, issues, and instructional implications. In B.F.
Jones and L. Idol (Eds.) Dimensions of thinking and cognitive instruction
(pp. 265 – 290). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Stiff, L.V., Johnson, J.L. & Johnson, M.R. (1993). Cognitive issues in
mathematics education. In P.S. Wilson (Ed), Research ideas for the
classroom: High school mathematics (pp. 3 – 20). New York, NY: National
Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Swanson, H. L. (1990). Influence of metacognitive knowledge and aptitude
on problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(2), 306 –
314.
Szetela, W. & Nicol, C. (1992). Evaluating problem solving in
mathematics. Educational Leadership, 42 – 45.
Wilson, J.W., Fernandez, M.L. & Hadaway, N. (1993). Mathematical problem
solving. In P.S. Wilson (Ed), Research ideas for the classroom: High
school mathematics (pp 57 – 78). New York, NY: National Council of
Teachers of Mathematics.
Appendix A: Cognitive-Metacognitive Framework
for Mathematical Performance (Garafalo & Lester, 1985)
Orientation: Strategic behavior to assess and understand a problem
Comprehension strategies
Analysis of information and conditions
Assessment of familiarity with task
Assessment of level of difficulty and chances of success
Organization: Planning of behavior and choice of actions
Identification of goals and subgoals
Global planning
Local planning (to implement global plans)
Execution: Regulation of behavior to conform to plans
Performance of local actions
Monitoring of progress of local and global plans
Trade-off decisions (eg. Speed vs. accuracy)
Verification: Evaluation of decisions made and outcomes of executed
plans
Evaluation of orientation and organization
Adequacy of representation
Adequacy of organizational decisions
Consistency of local plans with global plans
Appendix B: Set of 10 Teaching
Actions to Guide the Teacher during Classroom Problem Solving Lessons.
Teaching Action - 1
Read the problem to the class or have a student read the problem. Discuss
words or phases students may not understand
Purpose: Illustrate the importance of reading problems carefully and
focus on words that have special interpretations in mathematics.
Teaching Action – 2
Use a whole class discussion about understanding the problem. Use problem
specific comments and/or the problem solving guide.
Purpose: Focus attention on important data in the problem and clarify
parts of the problem
Teaching Action – 3
(Optional) Use a whole class discussion about possible solution
strategies. Use the problem solving guide.
Purpose: elicit ideas for possible ways to solve the problem
Teaching Action – 4
Observe and question students to determine where they are in the problem
solving process
Purpose: Diagnose students’ strengths and weaknesses related to problem
solving.
Teaching Action – 5
Provide hints as needed
Purpose: help students past blockages in solving a problem
Teaching Action – 6
Provide problem extensions as needed.
Purpose: challenge the early finishers to generalize their solution
strategy to a similar problem
Teaching Action – 7
Require students who obtain a solution to "answer the question".
Purpose: require students to look over their work and make sure it makes
sense
Teaching Action – 8
Show and discuss solutions using the problem solving guide as a basis for
discussion
Purpose: show and name different strategies used to successfully find a
solution
Teaching Action – 9
Relate the problem to previously solved problems and discuss or have
students solve extensions of the problem
Purpose: demonstrate that problem solving strategies are not problem
specific and that they help students recognize different kinds of situations
in which particular strategies may be useful.
Teaching Action – 10
Discuss special features of the problem such as picture accompanying the
problem statement.
Purpose: show how the special features of problem may influence how one
thinks about a problem.
Appendix C: An 8-Point System for Scoring Responses to Process Problems
Score Intrepretation
Orientation (understanding the problem)
0 Completely misinterprets the problem. (No work shown to
indicate understanding the problem.)
1 Misinterprets part of the problem. (Only one relevant piece of
information is used in a problem having more than one relevant piece
of information.)
2 Complete understanding of the problem. (Working shows complete
understanding of the problem.)
Organization (planning and choosing actions)
0 No attempt or a totally inappropriate plan. (No evidence of planning.)
1 Partly correct plan based on part of the problem interpreted
correctly. (Evidence of partly correct planning.)
2 A plan that could lead to a correct solution if implemented correctly.
Execution (regulating behavior to conform to plans)
0 No attempt or a totally inappropriate execution of a plan. ( No
evidence of execution or inappropriate execution of a plan.)
1 Evidence of a partial correct attempt to execute a plan.
2 Evidence of a complete execution of a plan/
Verification (evaluating decisions and outcomes)
0 No answer or wrong answer based on an inappropriate plan.
1 Copying error; computational error; partial answer for problem
with multiple answers; answer labeled incorrectly.
Correct answer in a complete sentence.
|